link.springer.com

The simple view of reading - Reading and Writing

  • ️Gough, Philip B.
  • ️Fri Jun 01 1990

References

  • Anderson, R., Hiebert, E., Scott, J. and Wilkinson, I. 1985. Becoming a nation of readers: The report of the Commission on Reading. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Applebee, A., Langer, J., and Mullis, I. 1987. Learning to be literate in America. Princeton, NJ: National Assessment of Educational Progress, Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R. 1984. Beginning reading instruction: From debate to reformation. In P. Pearson (ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 545–581). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, G. and Dykstra, R. 1967. The cooperative research program in first-grade reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly 2, 5–142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. 1954. Auding as the primary language ability. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.

  • Calfee, R. and Associates. 1978. Stanford foundation skills test. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. and Associates. 1980. Stanford foundation skills test (rev. ed.) Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. and Calfee, K. 1979. Interactive reading assessment system. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. and Calfee, K. 1981. Interactive reading assessment system (rev. ed.). Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. and Drum, P. 1986. Research on teaching reading. In M. Wittrock (ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 804–849). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. 1967. Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. 1980. Development of components of reading skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 656–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danks, J. 1980. Comprehension in listening and reading: Same or different? In F. Murray (ed.), Reading and understanding (pp. 1–39). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doehring, D., Trites, R., Patel, P., and Fiedorowicz, C. 1981. Reading disabilities: The interaction of reading, language, and neuropsychological deficits. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flesch, R. 1981. Why Johnny still can't read. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, S. and Calfee, R. 1984. Understanding and comprehending. Written Communication, 1, 459–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fries, C. 1963. Linguistics and reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gates, A. 1949. Character and purposes of the yearbook. In N. Henry (ed.), The forty-eighth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education: Part II. Reading in the elementary school (pp. 1–9). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glushko, R. 1979. The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. 1983. Context, form, and interaction. In K. Rayner (ed.), Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes (pp. 203–211). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. 1984. Word recognition. In P. Pearson (ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 225–253). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. and Hillinger, M. 1980. Learning to read: an unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. and Tunmer, W. 1986. Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J., Martuza, V., and Seifert, M. 1979. Impacts of instructional time in reading. In L. Resnick and P. Weaver (eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 3, pp. 153–178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harman, D. 1987. Illiteracy: A national dilemma. New York: Cambridge Book Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Healy, J. 1982. The enigma of hyperlexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 17, 319–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. 1982. Orthography and word recognition in reading. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., Calfee, R., and Mace-Matluck, B. 1984a. Teaching reading to bilingual children study: Vol. 5. Reading growth (Final Report, Contract No. 400-83-0007). Washington, DC: National Institute of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., Calfee, R., and Mace-Matluck, B. 1984b. Teaching reading to bilingual children study: Vol. 6. Instruction (Final Report, Contract No. 400-83-0007). Washington, DC: National Institute of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huey, E. 1968. The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge: MIT Press. (Original work published 1908).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M. and McClelland, J. 1979. Processing determinants of reading speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 151–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. and Baumann, J. 1984. Word identification. In P. Pearson (ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 583–608). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A. and Share, D. 1983. Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. 1977. From utterance to text: The bias of language in speech and writing. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 257–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., MacLeod, C., Hunt, E. and Davidson, J. 1985. Information processing correlates of reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 59–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, P., Hansen, J., and Gordon, C. 1979. The effect of background knowledge on young children's comprehension of explicit and implicit information. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11, 201–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. and Curtis, M. 1986. Reading. In R. Dillon and R. Sternberg (eds.), Cognition and instruction (pp. 13–57). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, A. 1980. A theoretical taxonomy of the differences between oral and written language. In R. Spiro, B. Bruce, and W. Brewer (eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 411–438). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. 1977. Toward an interactive model of reading. In S. Dornic (ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 573–603). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. and Porpodas, C. 1980. Lexical and non-lexical processing of spelling in dyslexia. In U. Frith (ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 443–473). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. and Crouse, J. 1981. The relationship of context-use skills to reading: A case for an alternative experimental logic. Child Development, 52, 1326–1329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. 1980. The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 294–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1980. Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1986. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A., and Feeman, D. 1984. Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., Nathan, R., and Vala-Rossi, M. 1986. Developmental changes in the cognitive correlates of reading ability and the developmental lag hypothesis. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 267–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. and West, R. 1983. On priming by a sentence context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sticht, T. and James, J. 1984. Listening and reading. In P. Pearson (ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 293–317). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. 1979. Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R., Kaestle, C., and Sum, A. 1987. The subtle danger: Reflections on the literacy abilities of America's young adults. Priceton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. 1985. The case for explicit decoding instruction. In J. Osborn, P. Wilson, and R. Anderson (eds.), Reading education: Foundations for a literate America (pp. 205–213). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

Download references