Cerebellar Gray Matter Volume, Executive Function, and Insomnia: Gender Differences in Adolescents - Scientific Reports
- ️Song, Chan-Hee
- ️Tue Jan 29 2019
Introduction
Motor function has been the main point of interest in cerebellar function1,2. After Leiner et al.3 suggested that the cerebellum is involved in mental skills such as learning, cognition, and language, subsequent studies4,5,6,7,8 have revealed that the cerebellum is also a major site responsible for high order cognitive processing (executive function, EF)7,8. The fact that the cerebellum enlarged at a slow rate during the human evolution, and that it is relatively large compared to other mammalian species supports this view9.
The brain region responsible for cognitive processing is not limited to a specific localization, but widely distributed in the cortical and subcortical areas10,11. The lateral prefrontal cortex is the critical area for EF and is associated closely with the cerebellum12. The cerebellum forms a corticocerebellar loop with the cortex responsible for EF. Information from the cortex is transferred to the cerebellum (especially the ventral dentate nucleus)13,14 via the pons, and feedback is provided the cortex via the thalamus15.
Studies based on neuropsychological tests16,17 and neuroimaging18,19,20,21 have shown impaired cognitive function in patients with cerebellar disability. Schmahmann and Sherman have defined cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome as a cerebellar lesion accompanied by EF, spatial cognition, personality change, and language deficit22.
Previous studies have suggested functional topography of the cerebellum related to various cognitive domains. Based on fMRI studies, Stoodley et al. have reported that the regions responsible for motor, cognition, and affect in the cerebellum are all different23,24,25,26,27,28,29. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies have revealed differences in cerebellar regional gray matter volume (GMV) dependent on cognitive domains, and showed that larger GMV results in enhanced cognitive function23,24,25,26,27,28,29. Nadkarni et al. reported that gait speed was faster and information-processing ability was better in larger cerebellar GMV regions associated with cognitive function30, while Miller et al. observed that the GMVs of the cerebellar vermis VI, V, VIII, IX, X regions are correlated with cognitive ability24. In addition, the correlation between cerebellar GMV and cognitive ability shows gender difference, with males having a higher correlation strength26. In a nutshell, all these clinical, functional and morphological studies support the fact that the cerebellum is involved in cognitive function.
The cerebellum is also closely associated with sleep31. Cerebellar activity is diminished in several sleep disorders, and various sleep problems accompany cerebellar lesions32,33,34. In cerebellar ataxia, sleep disorders such as Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder, restless legs syndrome, periodic leg movement disorder, excessive daytime sleepiness, insomnia, and obstructive sleep apnea also appear frequently depending on the area of cerebellar degeneration32,33,35,36,37. Several resting-state fMRI studies in adult insomnia patients have shown decreased regional connectivity in the right anterior cerebellar lobe38,39. If cognitive impairment is present, default mode network (DMN) and posterior cerebellar connectivity is also disrupted40, suggesting that the cerebellum is closely associated with both insomnia and cognitive dysfunction. Insomnia is the most common sleep disorder in adolescents, with a lifetime prevalence of 10.7% in USA 13–16 year-olds according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV), and most of them show a trend towards becoming chronic41,42. This may have significant consequences on EF responsible for daily life and education in this age period43,44.
As frontal lobe myelination continues to 15 years of age, brain development occurs very actively in the youth period45. Cerebellar volume increases in an inverted U shape according to age, and the peak shows gender difference (11.8, 15.6 in female, male, respectively), with late development in boys, but on the other hand, the total volume is greater by 10–13%46.
EF develops fast during early and middle childhood, and slows abruptly during late childhood and adolescence47 and this also shows gender difference43,47.
According to previous studies, the cerebellum is an important region responsible for adolescent cognitive function and sleep, and their correlation is expected to show different patterns depending on age and gender. Unfortunately, most of these studies have been performed in adults. One important difference between adult and adolescent brains is cerebellar neural pruning which proceeds until puberty48,49. It is not yet known exactly how the cerebellum’s neural pruning progresses, but considering the fact that the cerebellar neurons account for 80% of the total brain neural cells50, they will be far more affected by pruning than other brain regions. The adolescent cerebellum is affected actively by neural pruning, and in regard to the cerebellar regional volume and cognitive processing, it may be significantly different from that of an adult. As far as we know, this study is the first one to look at the GMV of the cerebellum, cognitive function, and sleep problem in adolescents at the same time. With this background in mind, the present study measures the regional GMV of the cerebellum, EF, and insomnia, and attempts to identify their correlation and gender differences in adolescents.
Results
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants
The girls were younger (14.25 ± 1.03 vs 15.23 ± 1.50, P < 0.006), and showed better performance on the IQ test (P < 0.036) and total errors (TE) (P < 0.040), a subcategory of Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST), but these differences were not significant after adjusting for age (Table 1).
Correlation analysis of regional gray matter volume, executive function, and insomnia severity index
We determined the cerebellar regions with significant correlation (P < 0.05, controlled for age and gender) between each subcategory of WCST and GMV (Fig. 1).
Partial correlation between cerebellar gray matter volume and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test scores (controlled for age and gender).
In boys (Fig. 2), positive correlations (controlled for age) were observed in the following: TE and lobule 3 (left, right), 4–5 (left), 7b (left), 9 (right), 10 (right), vermis 1–2, 3, 10; Perseverative Responses (PR), Perseverative Errors (PE) and lobule 3 (left, right), vermis 1–2, 3, 10; Nonperseverative Errors (NPE) and lobule 3 (left, right), 4–5 (left, right), 6 (left), 7b (left), 8 (left), 9 (left, right), 10 (right) vermis 1–2, 3, 8, 10; Conceptual Level Responses (CLR) and lobule 3 (left), vermis 1–2, 10; Categories Completed (CC) and lobule 3 (left), vermis 1–2, 3, 9, 10; Learning to Learn (LL) and vermis 1–2, 10. Failure to Maintain Set (FMS) and lobule 3 (left, right), 4–5 (left), vermis 1–2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9 showed negative correlations. No significant correlation was observed with Trials to Complete 1st Category (TCF).
Partial correlation between cerebellar gray matter volume and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test scores in boys (controlled for age).
In girls (Fig. 3), positive correlations (controlled for age) were observed in the following: TE and vermis 6; PR, PE and lobule 3 (left, right), vermis 6; CC and lobule 3 (left, right); TCF and lobule 7b (right), 8 (right), 9 (left), 10 (left, right); LL and lobule 7b (right), 8 (right), 9 (left, right), 10 (left), vermis 9. FMS and lobule 6 (left, right), 7af (left, right), 7at (left, right), vermis 4–5, 6 showed negative correlations. No significant correlations were observed with NPE and CC.
Partial correlation between cerebellar gray matter volume and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test scores in girls (controlled for age).
ISI showed negative correlation (controlled for age and gender) with GMV of lobule 10 (right) (Figs 1 and 4) in the total participants. In boys, insomnia severity index (ISI) showed negative correlation with GMV of lobule 10 (right) (Figs 2 and 4), and negative correlation (controlled for age) with WCST subcategories CLR, LL and CC (Table 2). In contrast, no significant correlation (controlled for age and gender) was observed in regional GMV or WCST subcategories in girls (Table 2 and Fig. 3).
Negative correlation between the volume at the right cerebellar lobe 10 and insomnia severity scale score (controlled for age and gender/controlled for age).
Based on previous studies showing that depressive symptoms have different effects on EF according to gender51,52, we reanalyzed our data after controlling for children’s depression inventory (CDI) total scores as covariates. However, our results did not change significantly in either boys or girls (Supplementary tables).
Discussion
In the present study, the correlation strength between WCST subcategories and cerebellar GMV differed by cerebellar region. The degree of correlation also varied according to gender. Far more overall correlations with cerebellar regions were observed in boys, with corresponding correlation strength being higher, and differences in localization were also observed in contrast to girls.
The correlations between cerebellar regional GMV and WCST subcategories showed that EF was better with larger GMV. It has been previously known that the sensorimotor domain corresponds to the anterior cerebellar lobe, part of lobule 6 in the posterior cerebellar lobe, and lobule 8, while the cognitive domain corresponds to part of lobule 6 in the posterior cerebellar lobe, and lobule 753. But we have found slightly different results regarding the cognition -related areas presented in these previous studies. Several factors, especially differences in EF test tool, and characteristics of participants, have to be considered when comparing our study with previous studies.
First, most previous studies have used different EF tests other than WCST, so this factor may have contributed partially to the differences observed in our study. The WCST used in this study is commonly recognized as a representative test reflecting EF and is used widely, but detailed differentiation is limited in some cases54. Previous studies have reported that damage to regional brain areas not considered to be directly involved in EF caused impairment of WCST performance55. In other words, this suggests that various regions of the brain are likely to be involved in WCST performance due to the complexity of the WCST performance itself. As a good example in explaining it, activations of primary motor lateral premotor and supplementary motor area responding to visuo-spatial cues and causing motor response and cerebellum were remarkable in a positron emission tomography (PET) study performed with WCST56,57. In addition, Nagahama et al. performed abbreviated WCST to exclude the effect of maintenance of sets, and it showed that attention to color was correlated with the activation of the ventro-occipital and left superior occipital cortex, while attention to shape was correlated with the inferior occipital cortex respectively56.
The GMV of the anterior cerebellum was highly correlated with the WCST subcategories in this study. Interestingly, previous functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated prominent activation of the anterior cerebellum during WCST performance, although the anterior cerebellum is not considered to be highly involved in EF55,56,58.
Second, these previous studies have been done mostly in adults, while our study was done in adolescents. Although this study is cross-sectional, the fact that the different ages of the participants produce different results can be inferred indirectly by comparing the differences in development patterns of the cerebellum between boys and girls. As we mentioned earlier, the cerebellar volume changes according to age in an inverted U-shape46,59,60, and the cerebellum is also a part of the brain which has big differences in development depending on gender59. The average age of the boys participating in this study was about 15 years and it has been known that the cerebellar volume in this age reaches the peak. On the other hand, the average age of the girls participating in this study was about 14 years, and the cerebellum volume starts to decrease after 2 years of the peak period.
In addition, for girls, the relationship between the anterior and inferior lobe tends to get weaker with advancing age, but for boys, this relationship doesn’t change46. Among the cerebellar regions, the inferior posterior lobe is most closely related to the frontal gray matter, which is the core area of EF, and develops in a pattern similar to that of the frontal gray matter46. Regional differences for EF in the cerebellum might be affected by the frontal gray matter, and this may explain the reason for a different cerebellar change between girls and boys with advancing age. The age of 14–15 years is still the period for active growth of frontal gray matter, and the development potential for EF is very strong.
Furthermore, most studies on both cerebellum and cognitive function have been previously performed in adolescents with either brain disorders involving the cerebellum or cerebellar injury61. On the other hand, some longitudinal studies on the cerebellar volume have been performed in typically developing adolescents46,59,60. This means that there exists some neuroimaging evidence for typically developing cerebellum in adolescents. However, none of those studies used tools that measure EF directly. So we can only posit that certain areas of the cerebellum play a specific role in EF by integrating the results from studies on cerebellar volume of typically developing adolescents and the results from studies on EF in adolescents with cerebellar dysfunction. Therefore, the results on the role of specific areas in the cerebellum of adolescents are not definitive.
Previous studies have suggested that the vermis is connected to the limbic system and functions as a limbic domain associated with emotion control62,63,64,65,66,67. The characteristic feature of this study is that FMS showed significant negative correlation with GMV of vermis 1–4 in boys. Additionally, GMV of vermis 5–9, although not significant, showed negative correlation, implying that selectively it is more closely related to the vermis than the other subcategories. It is also suggested that childhood depression is associated with FMS68.
After correcting for CDI, only a slight, but not significant increase was observed in correlation between FMS and GMV of vermis. This study was done with a cross sectional design, so it is not possible to determine the causal relationship of depression with EF and the cerebellar GMV. However, depression does not seem to have affected the correlation between FMS and GMV of the vermis. On the other hand, in girls, the FMS showed significant correlation with vermis 4–5, 6 and Crus I, II, showing a different pattern according to region from that in boys.
ISI showed negative correlation with the GMV of right lobule 10 (flocculonodular lobe) only in boys, and among WCST subcategies, CLR, LL, and CC showed negative correlation. However, the GMV of the flocculonodular lobe did not show correlation with these 3 categories, so we suggest that the correlation between ISI and the right flocculonodular lobe is independent from EF. In girls, ISI showed no significant correlation with any cerebellar GMV or WCST subcategories. Previous studies have also shown that the cognitive functions of boys are more affected by sleep than girls43. Previously, Joo et al.69 have reported in a VBM study of chronic primary insomnia patients that although cerebellar gray matter deficits are observed, the relationship between sleep and clinical variables associated with cognitive disorders are not clear. The flocculonodular lobe is only known to be involved in vestibular function and influences spatial learning70, and so far its relationship with sleep is not known. The regional function of the cerebellum is still mainly unknown. The correlation between GMV of the right flocculonodular lobe and insomnia that is only observed in boys in the present study requires further verification.
The correlation between cerebellar regional GMV and EF showed a large gender difference. Cerebellar regions that showed significant correlation with GMV and WCST subcategories were more numerous in boys (52, 37 when FDR < 0.2, versus 28, 0 when FDR < 0.2 in girls). Additionally, in boys, the areas of correlation were mainly centered on lobule 3, vermis 1–2, 3, 10, while in girls there was no particular region, and correlation strength was also relatively weak. There is no existing study that explains the reason for this sexual dimorphism. A plausible explanation may be the aforementioned differences in cerebellar development according to gender. Considering the fact that girls show earlier brain development71, the pruning period of adolescence may also be chronologically different, thus resulting in functional and formative differences dependent on gender.
Another possible explanation is the influence of sex hormones. There are many estrogen receptors in the cerebellum, especially in Purkinje neurons72,73, and the neuroprotective or neurotrophic effect of estrogen may affect EF and cerebellar volume. In studies with postmenopausal women, women receiving estrogen showed larger GMV in the cerebellum and cognitive function was better than the control group74,75,76. Another study with 12 to 27-year old participants found that in male subjects, higher endogenous testosterone corresponded with larger cerebellar GMV. Considering the results of previous studies, it is plausible that sex hormones may have contributed to the gender differences observed in the present study77.
However, as everything mentioned above at present is only presumed, further neurobiological studies are required in the future, in order to identify the root causes of the gender difference influence on the correlation between the cerebellar regional GMV and EF.
Limitations in this study include the small number of subjects, the fact that the total brain volume or white matter has not been taken into account, and the problem with WCST itself as a valid EF measurement tool. Also, because this study is a cross-sectional study, it cannot explain the causal relationship between cerebellar GMV and EF, and it is also limited in that we cannot be sure that every functional change accompanies a corresponding morphologic change (or vice versa).
In a nutshell, larger cerebellar GMV corresponded to better EF in adolescents. Insomnia did not influence these correlations, but more severe insomnia in boys correlated with smaller GMV in the right flocculonodular lobe. We suggest that the adolescent cerebellum is involved differently in EF according to gender.
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited from one middle school and one senior high school in Seoul, South Korea. Following approval by the school principals, the investigators visited the schools explaining the purpose of the study to the students and teachers. We also mailed letters to the parents outlining the study’s objectives, guaranteeing confidentiality, providing a contact telephone number for the principal investigator, and indicating that the parents would be informed of the results after the analyses were completed. The letter also included a statement that the parents were free to refuse to respond if they did not agree with the study’s objective.
A total of 64 students between the 7th and 11th grade (age range: 12–17 years old) volunteered to participate in this study. The Institutional Review Board (IRB) for Human Subjects at Seoul National University Hospital approved the study, and participants and their parents provided informed consent prior to enrollment. Of the 64 students who participated in the study, 9 were excluded due to incomplete responses, which resulted in a total of 55 subjects (31 boys, 24 girls). The mean age ± standard deviation (SD) of the 55 subjects was 14.80 ± 1.39 years.
Measures
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
The computerized WCST78 was used to measure executive function in this study. Participants were required to match response cards to the four stimulus cards along three dimensions (color, form, and number). The program calculated T scores (M = 50, SD = 10) for Total Errors (TE), Perseverative Responses (PR), Perseverative Errors (PE), Nonperseverative Errors (NPE), and Conceptual Level Responses (CLR) and raw scores for Categories Completed (CC), Trials to Complete 1st Category (TCF), Failure to Maintain Set (FMS), and Learning to Learn (LL). TE measured the total number of perseverative and non-perseverative errors. PR indicated the number of responses that were perseverative, regardless of the feedback. PE measured the inability to change reaction, indicating a tendency towards perseveration. NPE reflected the number of errors that were not perseverative. CLR was the proportion of continuously correct reactions in more than three times. CC was the number of times 10 correct responses consecutively were made. Trials to 1st Category represented the total number of trials needed to complete the first category successfully. FMS was the number of times participants made an error after 5–9 correct responses consecutively, indicating an efficiency of sorting. LL is mean changes in conceptual efficiency across consecutive categories. With FMS, lower scores reflect higher function of each subscale, whereas with the other categories including the subscales calculated by T scores, higher scores indicate higher function of each subscale.
Insomnia Severity Index (ISI)
The ISI was developed by Morin and colleagues78 and has been used for the last 20 years. It is a 7-item scale assessing the perceived severity of insomnia symptom (initial, middle, and terminal), the degree of satisfaction with sleep, the interference with daytime functioning, the awareness of impairment, and the concern caused by the sleep problems79. The respondents use a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 to 4 to describe their insomnia over the past 2 weeks.
The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI)
The CDI is a scale that has been modified from Beck’s depression inventory for children, and translated into Korean80. The CDI evaluates the level of depression by self-report from children and adolescents 7 to 17 years old. It consists of 27 items, and each item is rated from 0 to 2 points according to severity. The total score ranges from 0 to 54 points, and a higher score implies a more severe level of depression81.
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) data acquistion
MRI images were acquired using a 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner (Siemens, Magnetom Tim-Trio). High-resolution T1-weighted images were obtained from each participant using a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) pulse sequence (repetition time (TR) = 1900 ms; echo time (TE) = 3.13 ms; flip angle = 9°; matrix size = 256 × 256; Field-of-view (FOV) = 230 × 230 mm2; thickness = 0.9 mm). Head motion-related artifacts during scanning were minimized by using foam pads.
Regional Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) Analysis
We first inspected all T1-weighted images visually for motion and/or other artifacts and did not find any abnormality. All T1-weighted images were spatially preprocessed using canonical VBM-DARTEL procedure implemented in statistical parametric mapping (SPM12, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK)82. The procedure includes the following steps: (1) manual reorientation to place the anterior commissure at the origin and the anterior-posterior commissure in the horizontal plane, (2) gray matter segmentation using the SPM standard tissue probability map, (3) creation of a study-specific GM template from all the images using the DARTEL procedure, (4) spatial normalization of the individual GM images using DARTEL registration and modulation using the Jacobian determinant to adjust for nonlinear differences in individual brain size, and (5) spatial smoothing with Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM). After preprocessing, regional GMV values at the cerebellum were extracted by averaging the values within 26 cerebellar sites (8 vermis regions and 9 lobule regions in each hemisphere), which are defined by automated anatomical labeling83.
Statistical analysis
We performed partial correlation analyses between regional GMV in the cerebellum and several clinical scores representing WCST and ISI for boys and girls separately (covariates; age, CDI).
Levels of statistical significance were decided as false discovery rate (FDR) < 0.2 and FDR < 0.05, for controlling multiple correlation comparisons which were calculated among multiple regions and clinical variables. FDR levels of the range 0.1–0.2 are generally known to be acceptable for multiple comparison correction84. All statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB-based custom software (Mathworks, Sherborn, MA, USA).
Human and Animal Rights
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate
The Institutional Review Board (IRB) for Human Subjects at Seoul National University Hospital approved the study (No. C-1412-081-633), and participants and their parents provided informed consent prior to enrolment.
References
Glickstein, M. Motor skills but not cognitive tasks. Trends in neurosciences (Regular ed.) 16, 450–451; discussion 453 (1993).
Ito, M. Historical review of the significance of the cerebellum and the role of Purkinje cells in motor learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 978, 273–288 (2002).
Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L. & Dow, R. S. Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behavioral neuroscience 100, 443 (1986).
Doya, K. Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control. Current opinion in neurobiology 10, 732–739 (2000).
Houk, J. C. & Wise, S. P. Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. Cerebral cortex 5, 95–110 (1995).
Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. Neural Mechanisms for Interacting with a World Full of Action Choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience 33, 269–298, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409 (2010).
Bellebaum, C. & Daum, I. Cerebellar involvement in executive control. The Cerebellum 6, 184–192 (2007).
Ramnani, N. Frontal lobe and posterior parietal contributions to the cortico-cerebellar system. The Cerebellum 11, 366–383 (2012).
MacLeod, C. E., Zilles, K., Schleicher, A., Rilling, J. K. & Gibson, K. R. Expansion of the neocerebellum in Hominoidea. Journal of Human Evolution 44, 401–429 (2003).
Pezzulo, G. et al. Computational grounded cognition: a new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in psychology 3, 612 (2013).
Barsalou, L. Grounded cognition: past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science 2, 716-724 (2010).
Royall, D. R. et al. Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 14, 377–405 (2002).
Matano, S. Brief communication: proportions of the ventral half of the cerebellar dentate nucleus in humans and great apes. American Journal of Physical Anthropology: The Official Publication of the American Association of Physical Anthropologists 114, 163–165 (2001).
Kelly, R. M. & Strick, P. L. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. Journal of neuroscience 23, 8432–8444 (2003).
Heyder, K., Suchan, B. & Daum, I. Cortico-subcortical contributions to executive control. Acta psychologica 115, 271–289 (2004).
Schmahmann, J. D. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 16, 367–378 (2004).
Smith, E. E. & Jonides, J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283, 1657–1661 (1999).
Chen, S. A. & Desmond, J. E. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. Neuroimage 24, 332–338 (2005).
Kirschen, M. P., Chen, S. A., Schraedley-Desmond, P. & Desmond, J. E. Load-and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 24, 462–472 (2005).
Schumacher, E. H. et al. PET evidence for an amodal verbal working memory system. Neuroimage 3, 79–88 (1996).
Jonides, J. et al. Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363, 623–625 (1993).
Schmahmann, J. D. & Sherman, J. C. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 121, 561–579, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.4.561 (1998).
Stoodley, C., Valera, E. & Schmahmann, J. An fMRI study of intra-individual functional topography in the human cerebellum. Behavioural Neurology 23, 65–79 (2010).
Miller, T. et al. Cerebellar vermis size and cognitive ability in community-dwelling elderly men. The Cerebellum 12, 68–73 (2013).
Bernard, J. A., Leopold, D. R., Calhoun, V. D. & Mittal, V. A. Regional cerebellar volume and cognitive function from adolescence to late middle age. Hum Brain Mapp 36, 1102–1120, https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22690 (2015).
Hogan, M. J. et al. Cerebellar brain volume accounts for variance in cognitive performance in older adults. Cortex 47, 441–450, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.001 (2011).
Paul, R. et al. Relative contributions of the cerebellar vermis and prefrontal lobe volumes on cognitive function across the adult lifespan. Neurobiology of aging 30, 457–465 (2009).
MacLullich, A. M. J. et al. Size of the neocerebellar vermis is associated with cognition in healthy elderly men. Brain and cognition 56, 344–348 (2004).
Bernard, J. A. & Seidler, R. D. Relationships between regional cerebellar volume and sensorimotor and cognitive function in young and older adults. The Cerebellum 12, 721–737 (2013).
Nadkarni, N. et al. Association between cerebellar gray matter volumes, gait speed, and information-processing ability in older adults enrolled in the Health ABC study. The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences 69, 996–1003 (2014).
DelRosso, L. & Hoque, R. The cerebellum and sleep. Neurologic clinics 32, 893–900 (2014).
Pedroso, J. L. et al. Sleep disorders in cerebellar ataxias. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 69, 253–257 (2011).
Dang, D. & Cunnington, D. Excessive daytime somnolence in spinocerebellar ataxia type 1. J Neurol Sci 290, 146–147, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2009.12.007 (2010).
Reimold, M. et al. Spinocerebellar ataxia type 1, 2, and 3 and restless legs syndrome: striatal dopamine D2 receptor status investigated by [11C]raclopride positron emission tomography. Mov Disord 21, 1667–1673, https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20978 (2006).
Raggi, A. & Ferri, R. Sleep disorders in neurodegenerative diseases. European journal of neurology 17, 1326–1338 (2010).
Frenette, E. REM sleep behavior disorder. Medical Clinics 94, 593–614 (2010).
Strick, P. L., Dum, R. P. & Fiez, J. A. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci 32, 413–434, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125606 (2009).
Dai, X.-J. et al. Altered intrinsic regional brain spontaneous activity and subjective sleep quality in patients with chronic primary insomnia: a resting-state fMRI study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment 10, 2163 (2014).
Huang, S. et al. regional impairment of intrinsic functional connectivity strength in patients with chronic primary insomnia. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment 13, 1449 (2017).
Pang, R. et al. Aberrant Functional Connectivity Architecture in Participants with Chronic Insomnia Disorder Accompanying Cognitive Dysfunction: A Whole-Brain, Data-Driven Analysis. Front Neurosci 11, 259, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00259 (2017).
Johnson, E. O., Roth, T., Schultz, L. & Breslau, N. Epidemiology of DSM-IV insomnia in adolescence: lifetime prevalence, chronicity, and an emergent gender difference. Pediatrics 117, e247–e256 (2006).
Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R. & Duong, H. T. Chronic insomnia and its negative consequences for health and functioning of adolescents: a 12-month prospective study. Journal of Adolescent Health 42, 294–302 (2008).
Kuula, L. et al. Poor sleep and neurocognitive function in early adolescence. Sleep Med 16, 1207–1212, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.06.017 (2015).
Sadeh, A., Gruber, R. & Raviv, A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Development 73, 405–417 (2002).
Klingberg, T., Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Moseley, M. E. & Hedehus, M. Myelination and organization of the frontal white matter in children: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Neuroreport 10, 2817–2821 (1999).
Tiemeier, H. et al. Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla. Print) 49, 63–70 (2010).
Anderson, V. A., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R. & Catroppa, C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental neuropsychology 20, 385–406 (2001).
Thompson, R. A. & Nelson, C. A. Developmental science and the media: Early brain development. American Psychologist 56, 5 (2001).
Cowan, W. M., Fawcett, J. W., O’Leary, D. D. & Stanfield, B. B. Regressive events in neurogenesis. Science 225, 1258–1265 (1984).
Andersen, B. B., Korbo, L. & Pakkenberg, B. A quantitative study of the human cerebellum with unbiased stereological techniques. J Comp Neurol 326, 549–560, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903260405 (1992).
Davis, R. N. S. N.-H. Cognitive Inflexibility Among Ruminators and Nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research 24, 699–711 (2000).
Hankin, B. L. et al. Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of abnormal psychology 107, 128–140 (1998).
Stoodley, C. J. & Schmahmann, J. D. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. cortex 46, 831–844 (2010).
Greve, K. W., Brooks, J., Crouch, J. A., Williams, M. C. & Rice, W. J. Factorial structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 36, 283–285 (1997).
Berman, K. F. et al. Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia 33, 1027–1046 (1995).
Nagahama, Y. et al. Cerebral activation during performance of a card sorting test. Brain 119(Pt 5), 1667–1675 (1996).
Grafton, S. T., Mazziotta, J. C., Woods, R. P. & Phelps, M. E. Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115(Pt 2), 565–587 (1992).
Lie, C. H., Specht, K., Marshall, J. C. & Fink, G. R. Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage 30, 1038–1049, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.031 (2006).
Wierenga, L. et al. Typical development of basal ganglia, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum from age 7 to 24. Neuroimage 96, 67–72, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.072 (2014).
Brossard-Racine, M. & Limperopoulos, C. Normal Cerebellar Development by Qualitative and Quantitative MR Imaging: From the Fetus to the Adolescent. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 26, 331–339, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nic.2016.03.004 (2016).
Khan, A. J. et al. Cerebro-cerebellar Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry 78, 625–634, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.024 (2015).
Heath, R. G., Dempesy, C. W., Fontana, C. J. & Myers, W. A. Cerebellar stimulation: effects on septal region, hippocampus, and amygdala of cats and rats. Biological psychiatry (1969) 13, 501–529 (1978).
Schmahmann, J. D. An emerging concept: the cerebellar contribution to higher function. Archives of neurology 48, 1178–1187 (1991).
Schmahmann, J. D. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. Journal of Neurolinguistics 13, 189–214 (2000).
Babb, T., Mitchell, A. Jr. & Crandall, P. Fastigiobulbar and dentatothalamic influences on hippocampal cobalt epilepsy in the cat. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 36, 141–154 (1974).
Bobee, S., Mariette, E., Tremblay-Leveau, H. & Caston, J. Effects of early midline cerebellar lesion on cognitive and emotional functions in the rat. Behavioural brain research 112, 107–117 (2000).
Zanchetti, A. & Zoccolini, A. Autonomic hypothalamic outbursts elicited by cerebellar stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology 17, 475–483 (1954).
Kavanaugh, B. C. et al. Failure to maintain set as a predictor of childhood depression within a children’s psychiatric inpatient sample. Psychiatry research 246, 644–649 (2016).
Joo, E. Y. et al. Brain Gray Matter Deficits in Patients with Chronic Primary Insomnia. Sleep 36, 999–1007, https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2796 (2013).
Hitier, M., Besnard, S. & Smith, P. F. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front Integr Neurosci 8, 59, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00059 (2014).
Pesonen, A.-K. et al. Continuity and change in poor sleep from childhood to early adolescence. Sleep 37, 289–297 (2014).
Shughrue, P., Scrimo, P. & Merchenthaler, I. Estrogen binding and estrogen receptor characterization (ERα and ERβ) in the cholinergic neurons of the rat basal forebrain. Neuroscience 96, 41–49 (2000).
Tsutsui, K., Sakamoto, H., Shikimi, H. & Ukena, K. Organizing actions of neurosteroids in the Purkinje neuron. Neuroscience research 49, 273–279 (2004).
Ghidoni, R. et al. Effects of estrogens on cognition and brain morphology: involvement of the cerebellum. Maturitas 54, 222–228 (2006).
Rapp, S. R. et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. Jama 289, 2663–2672 (2003).
Jacobs, D. M. et al. Cognitive function in nondemented older women who took estrogen after menopause. Neurology 50, 368–373 (1998).
Schutter, D. J., Meuwese, R., Bos, M. G., Crone, E. A. & Peper, J. S. Exploring the role of testosterone in the cerebellum link to neuroticism: From adolescence to early adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology 78, 203–212 (2017).
Morin, C. M. Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. (Guilford Press, 1993).
Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A. & Morin, C. M. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep medicine 2, 297–307 (2001).
Cho, S.-C. & Lee, Y. Development of the Korean form of the Kovacs’ Children’s Depression Inventory. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 29, 943–956 (1990).
Kovacs, M. The Children’s Depression, Inventory (CDI). Psychopharmacology Bulletin 21, 995–998 (1985).
Friston, K., Tononi, G., Reeke, G. Jr, Sporns, O. & Edelman, G. M. Value-dependent selection in the brain: simulation in a synthetic neural model. Neuroscience 59, 229–243 (1994).
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289, https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 (2002).
Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A. & Nichols, T. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878 (2002).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean Government (NRF-2014R1A1A2057866 and NRF-2017R1D1A1B03031680). The funders did not have any role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Department of Psychiatry, St. Paul’s Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Kyu-In Jung, Min-Hyeon Park, Shin-Young Kim & Yae On Kim
Department of Biomedical Informatics, Ajou University School of Medicine, Suwon, Republic of Korea
Bumhee Park
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Bung-Nyun Kim
Department of Research Planning, National Center for Mental Health, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Subin Park
Department of Family Medicine, Yeouido St. Mary’s Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Chan-Hee Song
Office of Biostatistics, Ajou Research Institute for Innovative Medicine, Ajou University Medical Center, Suwon, Republic of Korea
Bumhee Park
Authors
- Kyu-In Jung
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Min-Hyeon Park
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Bumhee Park
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Shin-Young Kim
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Yae On Kim
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Bung-Nyun Kim
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Subin Park
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
- Chan-Hee Song
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Contributions
Author M.H.P. designed the study and wrote the protocol. Author K.I.J., B.P., B.N.K., S.Y.K., Y.O.K., S.P. and C.H.S. managed the literature searches and analyses. Authors M.H.P., B.P., S.Y.K. and C.H.S. undertook the statistical analysis, and author K.I.J., M.H.P. and B.P. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Correspondence to Min-Hyeon Park or Bumhee Park.
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Jung, KI., Park, MH., Park, B. et al. Cerebellar Gray Matter Volume, Executive Function, and Insomnia: Gender Differences in Adolescents. Sci Rep 9, 855 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-37154-w
Received: 23 July 2018
Accepted: 30 November 2018
Published: 29 January 2019
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-37154-w