nature.com

The effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior: a proposed mechanism for enduring antidepressant effects - npj Mental Health Research

  • ️Weissman, Cory R.
  • ️Tue Feb 20 2024
  • Review Article
  • Open access
  • Published: 20 February 2024

npj Mental Health Research volume 3, Article number: 7 (2024) Cite this article

Subjects

Abstract

Psilocybin is a serotonergic psychedelic shown to have enduring antidepressant effects. Currently, the mechanism for its enduring effects is not well understood. Empathy and prosocial behavior may be important for understanding the therapeutic benefit of psilocybin. In this article we review the effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior. Moreover, we propose that psilocybin may induce a positive feedback loop involving empathy and prosocial behavior which helps explain the observed, enduring antidepressant effects.

Similar content being viewed by others

Introduction

Psilocybin is a serotonergic psychedelic currently being considered for the treatment of several psychiatric illnesses including depression1,2,3,4,5,6. As psilocybin research rapidly expands, important questions about the mechanism by which psilocybin exerts its antidepressant effects remain unanswered. A question of particular interest is how psilocybin achieves enduring antidepressant effects well beyond the expected physiological metabolism of the drug. While the half-life of psilocybin is ~3 h7, antidepressant effects of a single dose have been shown to last for up to 6 months8,9. Animal and human studies of psilocybin have attempted to shed light on a mechanism by which psilocybin may be causing a therapeutic benefit. Animal models have shown that classic serotonergic psychedelics, including psilocybin, require 5HT2A receptor activity for psychoactive effects10. Further, preclinical studies have suggested that psilocybin may induce BDNF protein synthesis and subsequent downstream neuroplastic changes in a TRK-B receptor-dependent manner11

Human studies have confirmed the role of 5HT2A receptor agonism for the psychoactive effects of psilocybin12 and suggest that psilocybin may induce modulation of brain networks. For example, modulation of the default mode network has been associated with the therapeutic effects of psilocybin in human subjects13,14.

While research continues to investigate physiological mechanisms through which psilocybin induces therapeutic effects, many experts propose that the subjective psychedelic experience, often termed the mystical experience, is a necessary component15. Studies have demonstrated correlation between acute subjective mystical experiences and enduring positive psychological changes. For example, one study of healthy participants found that when high-dose psilocybin induced a mystical experience, it produced positive changes in interpersonal relations, gratitude, and life purpose at 6-month follow-up16. Similar findings were shown in another study that found psilocybin occasioned mystical experience produced positive changes in mood, attitude, behavior, and social effects at 3 months17. Another study found that psilocybin-induced mystical experience was associated with increases in trait mindfulness at 3-month follow-up18. Psilocybin-induced mystical experience has also been associated with reductions in depressive symptoms, as measured by the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptoms Self Report (QIDS-SR) at 5 weeks after psilocybin treatment19.

A greater sense of connectedness and unity with others is a key quality of the mystical experience and is thought to be involved in the antidepressant effect of psilocybin20. Greater sense of connected has also been linked with empathy and prosocial behavior21,22,23,24. Psilocybin has been shown to induce significant alterations in social processing; moreover, the effect of psilocybin on social functioning at an individual and community level may be vital for a comprehensive understanding of its mechanism of action and therapeutic potential25,26,27. Thus, it is worth considering empathy and prosocial behavior as processes implicated in the therapeutic effects of psilocybin. In this article, we review the literature on the effects of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior. Moreover, we suggest that psilocybin may catalyze a positive feedback loop involving empathy and prosocial behavior which, in part, may lead to enduring therapeutic benefit.

Exploring the relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior

There is no consensus definition of empathy; however, it can be broadly defined as the ability to adopt the perspective of others28,29. Empathy can be categorized as either cognitive empathy or emotional empathy30. Cognitive empathy is the mental process of understanding another individual’s perspective. It does not necessitate an affective response and is an analytical process. In contrast, emotional empathy is defined as one’s ability to recognize and feel the emotions of another. In other words, it is an induction of an affective state in response to the experience of others30. Emotional empathy can be further divided into positive and negative emotional empathy, defined as empathizing with the positive experiences in others versus empathizing with the distress of others, respectively31.

Research has explored neural correlates of empathy both in humans and animal models and various brain regions have been implicated. While not a comprehensive list, this includes the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, anterior cingulate, and insula32,33. One interesting finding is that differential activity appears to be dependent on context and affective valence (i.e., positive empathy vs. negative empathy). For example, positive empathy may be associated with activity in the medial orbitofrontal cortex, while negative empathy with activity in the right insulo-frontal cortex34. Furthermore, it appears there may be some overlap between empathy and psilocybin in regard to neural correlates. A study using a mouse model of fronto-temporal dementia demonstrated that loss of empathy was associated with hypo-excitability of the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex35. Empathy training in adolescents has been shown to cause increased activity in the temporoparietal junction. Interestingly, psilocybin has been associated with activity and increased neuroplasticity of the prefrontal cortex36,37, as well as in the temporal parietal junction38.

Prosocial behavior, defined as behavior by an individual that attempts to increase the welfare of another, is known to be catalyzed by empathy39. It has been thought that there is a strong, positive relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior29,40. However, some studies have brought this into question, such as when empathy induces overwhelming personal distress or prosocial efforts feel personally onerous, in which case empathy may not produce prosocial behavior28. These mixed findings suggest a more complex relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior which may be dependent on various mediating factors. In discussing the mechanism of action of psilocybin, it is worth exploring the role of positive affect in the relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior, as psilocybin has been shown to produce enduring positive affect16.

Some research suggests that positive affect can facilitate the relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior. One study examined gratitude, defined as a positive emotional response in recognition and appreciation of what is meaningful and valuable, often with thankfulness towards something outside oneself41. The study measured gratitude, empathy, and prosocial behavior, and found gratitude to be a mediating factor driving empathy induced prosociality42. Another study attempted to understand the role of affect in the “identifiable victim effect”. The “identifiable victim effect” refers to the tendency to help identifiable victims rather than anonymous ones. The study found that positive affective valence was associated with promoting the “identifiable victim effect.”43.

Researchers exploring the relationship between emotional empathy and prosocial behavior have proposed that positive emotional empathy may be a driver of prosocial behavior. One model of this relationship suggests that perceiving positive affect in others induces vicarious positive affect, which in turn leads to prosocial behaviors. This model asserts that prosocial behavior induces further positive affect29.

This model is supported by studies that suggest a positive feedback loop between positive affective states and prosocial behavior. One study examined the association between spending money on others and reported happiness. Participants were asked to recall a memory of spending money on themselves or on others and were subsequently asked to complete happiness measures. When the level of happiness was analyzed, it was found that those who recalled spending money on others displayed higher levels of happiness. Subsequently, participants were given the option to spend money on themselves or others, and it was found that those who were happier were more likely to spend money on others rather than themselves44. In another study, participants were asked to engage in a writing activity that was deemed to be positive in nature, with topics including gratitude, positive self-image, or recollection of an intensely positive experience; or, they were asked to write about a neutral topic. Participants were subsequently asked to perform kind acts. The study found that those who participated in positive writing reported increased effort in kind acts they performed; moreover, those that reported greater efforts in kind acts reported greater overall well-being. Another study found that gratitude-induced elevation in affect led to increased effort toward kind acts and that higher effort in kind acts led to further feelings of well-being45.

Taken together, the research suggests a complex relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior. It appears that positive affect may not only mediate this relationship, but catalyze a positive feedback loop with prosocial behavior.

The role of empathy in depression

Empathy may play an important role in the pathophysiology of depression as several studies have found significant association between depressive disorders and deficits in empathy46,47,48,49,50. For example, one study found that individuals with major depressive disorder displayed reduced cognitive and emotional empathy in comparison to control subjects48. In another study, participants with persistent depressive disorder displayed reduced compassion and social connection with others47. A systematic review of thirty-seven studies found empathy to be impaired in depression50. Further lending evidence that empathy may play a key role in depression is that therapeutic modalities which improve emotion recognition, empathic concern, and perspective taking such as mindfulness-based interventions, compassion cultivation training (CCT), and attachment-based compassion therapy (ABCT) demonstrate efficacy for the treatment of depression51,52,53,54,55,56,57.

Contrarily, some research suggests that empathy is positively correlated with depression58. This discrepancy may point to the heterogeneity of depression as an illness, with some presentations of depression, such as persistent depressive disorder, treatment-resistant depression, and childhood-onset depression manifesting with relative deficits in empathy59,60,61. It is possible that in some presentations of depression, empathy may not be a significant factor. In other presentations such as depression with concomitant anxiety, empathic response, particularly negative emotional empathy, may worsen distress62,63.

Psilocybin and empathy

Research suggests a link between psilocybin and empathy such that psilocybin may induce changes in specific subtypes of empathy. A double-blind placebo-controlled study showed that healthy volunteers who ingested psilocybin had acute increases in implicit emotional empathy, described as sharing an emotional state with others, and explicit emotional empathy, described as prosocial attitudes toward others, but displayed no changes in measures of cognitive empathy64. Importantly, psilocybin may induce increased empathy in the context of depression. For example, in participants with treatment-resistant depression, psilocybin ingestion has been shown to increase emotional facial processing, which is positively correlated with empathy65,66,67. While there is limited research available on the length of effect, some evidence suggests that increased emotional empathy may persist beyond the acute effects of psilocybin. For instance, one study found emotional empathy remained enhanced at seven days, and another suggested increased emotional empathic states at up to 6 months16,68.

It is worth noting that other serotonergic psychedelics, which have similar neurobiological and phenomenological mechanisms, have also been shown to enhance empathy. Lysergic acid diethylamide has been shown in at least two studies to enhance empathy and prosocial attitudes69,70. Ayuhuasca, a plant that contains the psychedelic compound dimethyltryptamine, has also been shown to increase emotional empathy71,72.

Psilocybin and prosocial behaviors

Some research has examined the association between psilocybin and prosocial behavior. Interest has been largely focused on changes in affective states and reductions in psychiatric symptoms. Behavioral change has not been well studied, with an important exception being substance use, which is an active area of research4,73. The current body of research appears to be mixed, with some studies suggesting psilocybin induces prosocial behavior and others showing this may not be the case.

Research in criminal behavior and recidivism suggests there may be an association between psilocybin and prosocial behavior. An observational study found that convicted felons who had a history of psychedelic use were more likely to comply with the rules of community corrective supervision. This sample population included those with a history of substance use and violent offense74. A prospective study exploring the relationship between psychedelic use and intimate partner violence in inmates found a lower incidence of intimate partner violence in individuals with a history of hallucinogen use75. Although the first study above did not specify which hallucinogens were used, the second found that 86.9% of study participants used classical, serotonergic psychedelics, including psilocybin. These naturalistic study designs can only show associations, but the results suggest that psilocybin and other psychedelics may be protective against antisocial behaviors.

Another interesting area of research explores the association of pro-environmental behaviors and psilocybin. While prosocial behaviors are generally thought to be those that are human to human in nature, prosocial behavior can also be actions benefiting non-human beings such as plants and animals.77Pro-environmental behavior can be categorized as a type of prosocial behavior given that it is not self-serving, and rather focuses on contributing positively to beings external to oneself (plants, animals, humans)76. In a survey study of 1487 participants, those who endorsed a lifetime history of classic psychedelic use including psilocybin, were more likely to endorse feelings of connectedness to nature and self-reported pro-environmental behaviors77. A later follow-up study showed that the potency of the psychedelic experience, specifically mystical experience as measured by the mystical experience questionnaire, was positively associated with self-reported pro-environmental behavior78.

Some studies, however, have shown lack of association between psilocybin and prosocial behavior. For example, one study examined both empathy and moral decision making after psilocybin ingestion. It was found that while emotional empathy was enhanced with psilocybin, moral decision making was unchanged64. Another study found that psilocybin decreased altruistic punishment, or punishment of others without personal gain, when participants were told their actions would affect another person rather than a computer79. While altruistic punishment can be considered prosocial, the reduction was assumed to be due to the social reward of minimizing punishment of another. In that sense, this finding could be interpreted as either inducing or inhibiting prosocial behavior.

These findings suggest that there may be a relationship between psilocybin and prosocial behavior, although this is certainly not definitive. It is possible that there is not a direct relationship between psilocybin and prosocial behavior and that activation of other psychological processes is required in order to observe an association. More robust studies conducted to explore psilocybin and prosocial behavior are necessary to further understand this relationship.

A proposed mechanism for the enduring effects of psilocybin

In exploring the persisting antidepressant effects of psilocybin, it is intriguing to consider how empathy and prosocial behavior may interact in leading to therapeutic effects. As this paper has summarized, research suggests that psilocybin may lead to increases in empathy and prosocial behavior. Moreover, while not the focus of this article, psilocybin has been shown to produce enduring positive affect16. We hypothesize a mechanism involving empathy and prosocial behavior in the context of positive affect which may induce a positive feedback loop, which in part, may lead to the enduring antidepressant effects of psilocybin (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1: Psilocybin may induce a positive feedback loop involving empathy, prosocial behavior, and positive affect.
figure 1

Psilocybin may lead to enhancement of both empathy and prosocial behaviors in the context of positive affect, inducing a positive feedback loop between positive affect and prosocial behavior that sustains long-term antidepressant effects from a single dose.

Full size image

This hypothesized model is similar to the one proposed by Telle et al. which was previously discussed29. The model proposed by Telle et al. suggests that positive emotional empathy can induce prosocial behavior, which then further leads to positive affect. It is possible that psilocybin induces positive emotional empathy, which may lead to prosocial behavior, followed by a feedback loop involving prosocial behavior and positive affect. This cycle may then be self-propagating, and able to sustain the therapeutic effects of psilocybin after a single dose. It is also possible that psilocybin induces empathy and positive affect distinctly, but simultaneously, which can also produce a similar self-propagating cycle.

It can be argued that positive affect may independently be driving the antidepressant effects of psilocybin, and that empathy and prosocial behavior may not be involved. It can also be argued that positive affect may induce prosocial behavior independent of empathy. While this may be possible, many substances that can produce positive affect, such as illicit amphetamines and alcohol, do not display enduring antidepressant effects, nor do they display positive social effects beyond acute intoxication80,81,82,83,84. Moreover, research suggests that even challenging experiences with psilocybin, in which immediate positive affective states are not present, can produce longitudinal positive affect and sustained therapeutic benefit85. Thus, while positive affect is likely a component of the therapeutic mechanism, other psychological factors are contributing. As aforementioned, feelings of connectedness with others, which is associated with empathy and prosociality, which have therapeutic value, lend evidence to these processes being involved in the therapeutic mechanism of psilocybin20,21,24.

Depression is a heterogeneous illness with certain presentations manifesting with deficits in empathy59,60,61. Hence, this hypothesized mechanism may be a catalyst for psychological change in specific presentations of depression where empathy is impaired. It is also possible that in other variants of depression where impairment of empathy is not present, psilocybin may produce effects through other potential mechanisms or the antidepressant effects of psilocybin may be attenuated.

Psilocybin is currently being studied in conjunction with psychotherapy, termed psilocybin-assisted therapy. However, the modality of psychotherapy used and even the need for a psychotherapy component have been brought into question86. Examining the effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior could produce vital information for how we can leverage psychological strategies to increase the therapeutic benefits of psilocybin. For example, if psychotherapeutic interventions are framed such as to encourage positive affect and empathy, they may be coupled with psilocybin and work in a synergistic manner. Some research already suggests that this may be the case87,88.

Understanding the effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior may also be useful in understanding therapeutic effects of psilocybin in other psychiatric disorders. For example, psilocybin has shown promise in the treatment of both obsessive–compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder and these disorders have been associated with deficits in empathy4,5,6,89,90. In that sense, empathy may be a transdiagnostic component of psilocybin’s therapeutic effect, a possibility that is worth further exploration.

Conclusion

In this article, we review the literature on empathy and prosocial behavior as it pertains to psilocybin. Currently, research on the effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior is limited. However, enough evidence exists to warrant further exploration of this area. Given the current body of evidence, we hypothesize a mechanism by which psilocybin may lead to enduring effects, in part, through a self-propagating feedback loop involving empathy, prosocial behavior, and positive affect. Further research into the effects of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior will help broaden our understanding of psilocybin and other psychedelics as a treatment for psychiatric illness.

References

  1. Raison, C. L. et al. Single-dose psilocybin treatment for major depressive disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 330, 843–853 (2023).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Goodwin, G. M. et al. Single-dose psilocybin for a treatment-resistant episode of major depression. N. Engl. J. Med. 387, 1637–1648 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Grob, C. S. et al. Pilot study of psilocybin treatment for anxiety in patients with advanced-stage cancer. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 71–78 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bogenschutz, M. P. et al. Percentage of heavy drinking days following psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy vs placebo in the treatment of adult patients with alcohol use disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 953–962 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Ching, T. H. W. et al. Safety, tolerability, and clinical and neural effects of single-dose psilocybin in obsessive–compulsive disorder: protocol for a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, non-crossover trial. Front. Psychiatry 14, 1178529 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Moreno, F. A., Wiegand, C. B., Taitano, E. K. & Delgado, P. L. PRESS, INC. Safety, tolerability, and efficacy of psilocybin in 9 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 67, 1735–1740 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Brown, R. T. et al. Pharmacokinetics of escalating doses of oral psilocybin in healthy adults. Clin. Pharmacokinet 56, 1543–1554 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Barrett, F. S. et al. Emotions and brain function are altered up to one month after a single high dose of psilocybin. Sci. Rep. 10, 2214 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Carhart-Harris, R. L. et al. Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: six-month follow-up. Psychopharmacology 235, 399–408 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. González-Maeso, J. et al. Cellular/molecular transcriptome fingerprints distinguish hallucinogenic and nonhallucinogenic 5-hydroxytryptamine 2A receptor agonist effects in mouse somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 23, 8836–8843 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Ly, C. et al. Psychedelics promote structural and functional neural plasticity. Cell Rep. 23, 3170–3182 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Quednow, B. B. et al. Psilocybin-induced deficits in automatic and controlled inhibition are attenuated by ketanserin in healthy human volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 630–640 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Barrett, F. S. et al. Psilocybin acutely alters the functional connectivity of the claustrum with brain networks that support perception, memory, and attention. Neuroimage 218, 116980 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Daws, R. E. et al. Increased global integration in the brain after psilocybin therapy for depression. Nat. Med. 28, 844–851 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Yaden, D. B. & Griffiths, R. R. The subjective effects of psychedelics are necessary for their enduring therapeutic effects. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 4, 568–572 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Griffiths, R. R. et al. Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Psychopharmacology 32, 49–69 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. McCulloch, D. E.-W. et al. Psilocybin-induced mystical-type experiences are related to persisting positive effects: a quantitative and qualitative report. Front. Pharmacol. 13, 841648 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Søndergaard, A. et al. Lasting increases in trait mindfulness after psilocybin correlate positively with the mystical-type experience in healthy individuals. Front. Psychol. 13, 948729 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Roseman, L., Nutt, D. J. & Carhart-Harris, R. L. Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Front. Pharmacol. 8, 974 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Zeifman, R. J. et al. How does psilocybin therapy work? An exploration of experiential avoidance as a putative mechanism of change. J. Affect Disord. 334, 100–112 (2023).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Acevedo, E. C. & Tost, J. Self-transcendent experience and prosociality: connecting dispositional awe, compassion, and the moral foundations. Pers. Individ. Dif. 214, 8069 (2023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Barrett, F. S., Johnson, M. W. & Griffiths, R. R. Validation of the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. J. Psychopharmacol. 29, 1182–1190 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Hood, R. W. The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. J. Scientific Study Rel. 14, 29–41 (1975).

  24. Piff, P. K. et al. Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 883–899 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Preller, K. H. et al. Effects of serotonin 2A/1A receptor stimulation on social exclusion processing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 5119–5124 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kraehenmann, R. et al. Psilocybin-induced decrease in amygdala reactivity correlates with enhanced positive mood in healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 572–581 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Roseman, L. et al. Editorial: Psychedelic sociality: pharmacological and extrapharmacological perspectives. Front. Pharmacol. 13, 979764 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Stevens, F. & Taber, K. The neuroscience of empathy and compassion in pro-social behavior. Neuropsychologia 159, 107925 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Telle, N. T. & Pfister, H. R. Positive empathy and prosocial behavior: a neglected link. Emot. Rev. 8, 154–163 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Decety, J. et al. Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos. Trans. R Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 371, 20150077 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Andreychik, M. R. & Migliaccio, N. Empathizing with others’ pain versus empathizing with others’ joy: examining the separability of positive and negative empathy and their relation to different types of social behaviors and social emotions. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 37, 274–291 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Beadle, J. N. & De La Vega, C. E. Impact of aging on empathy: review of psychological and neural mechanisms. Front. Psychiatry 10, 331 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Walter, H. Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: concepts, circuits, and genes. Emot. Rev. 4, 9–17 (2012).

  34. Lamm, C., Silani, G. & Singer, T. Distinct neural networks underlying empathy for pleasant and unpleasant touch. Cortex 70, 79–89 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Phillips, H. L. et al. Dorsomedial prefrontal hypoexcitability underlies lost empathy in frontotemporal dementia. Neuron 111, 797–806.e6 (2023).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Calder, A. E. & Hasler, G. Towards an understanding of psychedelic-induced neuroplasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 48, 104–112 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Tang, Z.-H. et al. The effects of serotonergic psychedelics in synaptic and intrinsic properties of neurons in layer II/III of the orbitofrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology 240, 1275–1285 (2023).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kral, T. R. A. et al. Neural correlates of video game empathy training in adolescents: a randomized trial. NPJ Sci. Learn 3, 13 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Luengo Kanacri, B. P. et al. Measuring prosocial behaviors: psychometric properties and cross-national validation of the prosociality scale in five countries. Front. Psychol. 12, 693174 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Aknin, L. B., Van de Vondervoort, J. W. & Hamlin, J. K. Positive feelings reward and promote prosocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 55–59 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Sansone, R. A. & Sansone, L. A. Gratitude and well being: the benefits of appreciation. Psychiatry 11, 18–22 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  42. Pang, Y. L., Song, C. & Ma, C. Effect of different types of empathy on prosocial behavior: gratitude as mediator. Front. Psychol. 13, 768827 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Genevsky, A. et al. Neural underpinnings of the identifiable victim effect: affect shifts preferences for giving. J. Neurosci. 33, 17188–17196 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Aknin, L. B., Dunn, E. W. & Norton, M. I. Happiness runs in a circular motion: evidence for a positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and happiness. J. Happiness Stud. 13, 347–355 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Layous, K. et al. What triggers prosocial effort? A positive feedback loop between positive activities, kindness, and well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 385–398 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Ferguson, H. J. & Cane, J. Tracking the impact of depression in a perspective-taking task. Sci. Rep. 7, 14821 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Frick, A. et al. Reduced social connectedness and compassion toward close others in patients with chronic depression compared to a non-clinical sample. Front. Psychiatry 12, 608607 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Cusi, A. M. et al. Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Res. 188, 231–236 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Weightman, M. J., Knight, M. J. & Baune, B. T. A systematic review of the impact of social cognitive deficits on psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder and opportunities for therapeutic intervention. Psychiatry Res. 274, 195–212 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M. & Aan Het Rot, M. Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J. Affect Disord. 150, 1–16 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Ernst, E., Rand, J. I. & Stevinson, C. Complementary therapies for depression: an overview. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 1026–1032 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Creswell, J. D. Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-042716-051139 (2017).

  53. Collado-Navarro, C. et al. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction and attachment-based compassion therapy for the treatment of depressive, anxious, and adjustment disorders in mental health settings: a randomized controlled trial. Depress Anxiety 38, 1138–1151 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Hansen, N. H. et al. Effect of a compassion cultivation training program for caregivers of people with mental Illness in Denmark: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw. Open 4, e211020 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Brito-Pons, G., Campos, D. & Cebolla, A. Implicit or explicit compassion? Effects of compassion cultivation training and comparison with mindfulness-based stress reduction. Mindfulness 9, 1494–1508 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Centeno, R. P. R. & Fernandez, K. T. G. Effect of mindfulness on empathy and self-compassion: an adapted MBCT program on Filipino college students. Behav. Sci. 10, 61 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Patel, S. et al. Curricula for empathy and compassion training in medical education: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 14, e0221412 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Yan, Z. et al. The dark side of empathy: meta-analysis evidence of the relationship between empathy and depression. Psych. J. 10, 794–804 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Guhn, A. et al. Impaired empathic functioning in chronic depression: behavioral evidence for the Cognitive Behavioral Analysis System of Psychotherapy (CBASP) model. J. Psychiatr. Res. 152, 79–85 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Rottenberg, J., Kovacs, M. & Yaroslavsky, I. Non-response to sad mood induction: implications for emotion research. Cogn. Emot. 32, 431–436 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Kilian, H. M. et al. Impaired socio-affective, but intact socio-cognitive skills in patients with treatment-resistant, recurrent depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 153, 206–212 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Pittelkow, M. M. et al. Social anxiety and empathy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Anxiety Disord. 78, 102357 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bray, K. O. et al. Associations between cognitive and affective empathy and internalizing symptoms in late childhood. J. Affect Disord. 290, 245–253 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Pokorny, T. et al. Effect of psilocybin on empathy and moral decision-making. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 747–757 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Beals, K., Sperry, S. H. & Sheffield, J. M. Empathy, emotion recognition, and paranoia in the general population. Front. Psychol. 13, 804178 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Clark, G. M. et al. The effect of empathy and context on face-processing ERPs. Neuropsychologia 147, 107612 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Stroud, J. B. et al. Psilocybin with psychological support improves emotional face recognition in treatment-resistant depression. Psychopharmacology 235, 459–466 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Mason, N. L. et al. Sub-acute effects of psilocybin on empathy, creative thinking, and subjective well-being. J. Psychoactive Drugs 51, 123–134 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Dolder, P. C. et al. LSD acutely impairs fear recognition and enhances emotional empathy and sociality. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2638–2646 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Holze, F. et al. Role of the 5-HT2A receptor in acute effects of LSD on empathy and circulating oxytocin. Front. Pharmacol. 12, 711255 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Kiraga, M. K. et al. Persisting effects of ayahuasca on empathy, creative thinking, decentering, personality, and well-being. Front. Pharmacol. 12, 721537 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Uthaug, M. V. et al. A placebo-controlled study of the effects of ayahuasca, set and setting on mental health of participants in ayahuasca group retreats. Psychopharmacology 238, 1899–1910 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Jones, G. et al. Associations between classic psychedelics and opioid use disorder in a nationally-representative U.S. adult sample. Sci. Rep. 12, 4099 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Hendricks, P. S. et al. Hallucinogen use predicts reduced recidivism among substance-involved offenders under community corrections supervision. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 62–66 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Walsh, Z. et al. Hallucinogen use and intimate partner violence: prospective evidence consistent with protective effects among men with histories of problematic substance use. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 601–607 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Pfattheicher, S., Nielsen, Y. A. & Thielmann, I. Prosocial behavior and altruism: a review of concepts and definitions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44, 124–129 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Forstmann, M. & Sagioglou, C. Lifetime experience with (classic) psychedelics predicts pro-environmental behavior through an increase in nature relatedness. J. Psychopharmacol. 31, 975–988 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Paterniti, K., Bright, S. & Gringart, E. The relationship between psychedelic use, mystical experiences, and pro-environmental behaviors. J. Humanist. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1177/00221678221111024 (2022).

  79. Gabay, A. S. et al. Psilocybin and mdma reduce costly punishment in the ultimatum game. Sci. Rep. 8, 8236 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. McKetin, R. et al. Mental health outcomes associated with of the use of amphetamines: a systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine 16, 81–97 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Wardle, M. C. & De Wit, H. Effects of amphetamine on reactivity to emotional stimuli. Psychopharmacology 220, 143–153 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Mat Hassan, N. et al. Association of prosocial behavior with ever smoking and alcohol drinking among school-going adolescents. Heliyon 6, e04530 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Hammerton, G. et al. Effects of excessive alcohol use on antisocial behavior across adolescence and early adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 857–865 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Sommers, I., Baskin, D. & Baskin-Sommers, A. Methamphetamine use among young adults: Health and social consequences. Addict. Behav. 31, 1469–1476 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Carbonaro, T. M. et al. Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1268–1278 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Goodwin, G. M. et al. Must psilocybin always “assist psychotherapy”? Am. J. Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.20221043 (2023).

  87. Heuschkel, K. & Kuypers, K. P. C. Depression, mindfulness, and psilocybin: possible complementary effects of mindfulness meditation and psilocybin in the treatment of depression. a review. Front Psychiatry 11, 224 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Pots, W. & Chakhssi, F. Psilocybin-assisted compassion focused therapy for depression. Front. Psychol. 13, 812930 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Kumar, L., Skrzynski, C. J. & Creswell, K. G. Meta-analysis of associations between empathy and alcohol use and problems in clinical and non-clinical samples. Addiction 117, 2793–2804 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Bora, E. Social cognition and empathy in adults with obsessive compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 316, 114752 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA

    Kush V. Bhatt & Cory R. Weissman

Authors

  1. Kush V. Bhatt

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  2. Cory R. Weissman

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Contributions

K.B. wrote the main manuscript. C.R.W. reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cory R. Weissman.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

K.B. declares no competing interests. C.R.W. declares no competing financial interests but currently serves as sub-investigator on a phase III trial on COMP360 in treatment-resistant depression by COMPASS Pathfinder Limited.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bhatt, K.V., Weissman, C.R. The effect of psilocybin on empathy and prosocial behavior: a proposed mechanism for enduring antidepressant effects. npj Mental Health Res 3, 7 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44184-023-00053-8

Download citation

  • Received: 23 August 2023

  • Accepted: 20 December 2023

  • Published: 20 February 2024

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44184-023-00053-8