link.springer.com

Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search - Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

  • ️Wolfe, Jeremy M.
  • ️Wed Jun 01 1994

References

  • Adelson, E. H., &Bergen, J. R. (1991). The plenoptic function and the elements of early vision. In M. Landy & J. A. Movshon (Eds.),Computational models of visual processing (pp. 3–20). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahissar, M., &Hochstein, S. (1992). Perceptual learning: Interactions between task and stimulus specificities.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1262. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Aks, D. J., &Enns, J. T. (1993). Early vision’s analysis of slant-from-texture.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1185. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Alkhateeb, W. F., Morris, R. J., &Ruddock, K. H. (1990). Effects of stimulus complexity on simple spatial discriminations.Spatial Vision,5, 129–141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allan, S. E., &Blough, D. S. (1989). Feature-based search asymmetries in pigeons and humans.Perception & Psychophysics,46, 456–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, G. J. (1990). Focused attention in three-dimensional space.Perception & Psychophysics,47, 112–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, G. J., &Kramer, A. F. (1993). Limits of focused attention in three-dimensional space.Perception & Psychophysics,53, 658–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andriesen, J. J., &Bouma, H. (1976). Eccentric vision: Adverse interactions between line segments.Vision Research,16, 71–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arguin, M., &Cavanagh, P. (1988). Parallel processing of two disjunctive targets.Perception & Psychophysics,44, 22–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. L., &Griggs, D. S. (1988). Age and visual search: Expanding the useful field of view.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,5, 2210–2219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, K. K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., &Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes among older drivers.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 3110–3123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, K. K., Roenker, D. L., &Bruni, J. R. (1990). Developmental changes in attention and visual search throughout adulthood. In J. T. Enns (Ed.),The development of attention: Research and theory (pp. 489–508). Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baylis, G. C., &Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 451–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Av, M. B., Sagi, D., &Braun, J. (1992). Visual attention and perceptual grouping.Perception & Psychophysics,52, 277–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, J. R., &Adelson, E. H. (1988). Early vision and texture perception.Nature,333, 363–364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, J. R., &Julesz, B. (1983). Rapid discrimination of visual patterns.IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, & Cybernetics,SMC-13, 857–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding.Psychological Review,94, 115–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R. (1989). A neural theory of binocular rivalry.Psychological Review,96, 145–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bravo, M., &Blake, R. (1990). Preattentive vision and perceptual groups.Perception,19, 515–522.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bravo, M. J., &Nakayama, K. (1992). The role of attention in different visual-search tasks.Perception & Psychophysics,51, 465–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breese, B. B. (1909). Binocular rivalry.Psychological Review,16, 410–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. M., Enns, J. T., &Greene, H. (1993). Preattentive processing of line junctions can be altered by perceptual set.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1234. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. M., Weisstein, N., &May, J. G. (1992). Visual search for simple volumetric shapes.Perception & Psychophysics,51,40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bülthoff, H. H., &Blake, A. (1989). Does the seeing brain know physics?Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,30, 262. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention.Psychological Review,97, 523–547.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen, C. (1991). Visual selection of features and objects: Is location special? A reinterpretation of Nissen’s (1985) findings.Perception & Psychophysics,50, 87–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen, C., &Pedersen, L. F. (1983). Color segregation and visual search.Perception & Psychophysics,33, 487–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callaghan, T. C. (1984). Dimensional interaction of hue and brightness in preattentive field segregation.Perception & Psychophysics,36, 25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, R. C. (1982). Visual search with color.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,8, 127–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanagh, P., Arguin, M., &Treisman, A. (1990). Effect of surface medium on visual search for orientation and size features.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 479–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cave, K. R., &Pashler, H. (1994a).Visual selection mediated by location 1: Selecting successive visual objects. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Cave, K. R., &Pashler, H. (1994b).Visual selection mediated by location 2: Selecting noncontiguous locations. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Cave, K. R., &Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search.Cognitive Psychology,22, 225–271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheal, M., &Lyon, D. (1989). Attention effects on form discrimination at different eccentricities.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,41A, 719–746.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheal, M., &Lyon, D. (1992). Attention in visual search: Multiple search classes.Perception & Psychophysics,52, 113–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chelazzi, L., Miller, E. K., Duncan, J., &Desimone, R. (1993). Neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex.Nature,363, 345–347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, I., &DeValois, R. L. (1993). Even and odd symmetric mechanisms are equally involved in texture discrimination.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1289. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, L. (1982). Topological structure in visual perception.Science,218, 699–700.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, L. (1990). Holes and wholes: A reply to Rubin and Kanwisher.Perception & Psychophysics,47, 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., &Wolfe, J. M. (1994).Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Chun, M. M., Wolfe, J. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992). Texture gradients group within but not across feature maps.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 960. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. (1993). Asymmetries in visual search for conjunctive targets.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 775–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., &Ivry, R. B. (1989). Illusory conjunction inside and outside the focus of attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 650–663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., &Ivry, R. B. (1991). Density effects in conjunction search: Evidence for a coarse location mechanism of feature integration.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 891–901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., &Rafal, R. D. (1991). Attention and feature integration: Illusory conjunctions in a patient with a parietal lobe lesion.Psychological Science,2, 106–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1989). Discriminability and dimensionality effects in visual search for featural conjunctions: A functional pop-out.Perception & Psychophysics,46, 72–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., Schein, S. J., Moran, J., &Ungerleider, L. G. (1985). Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex.Vision Research,25, 441–452.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., &Ungerleider, L. G. (1989). Neural mechanisms of visual processing in monkeys. In F. Boiler & J. Grafman (Eds.),Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 267–299). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dick, M. (1989).Parallel and serial processes in motion detection. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Weizmann Institute, Rehovot, Israel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dick, M., Ullman, S., &Sagi, D. (1987). Parallel and serial processes in motion detection.Science,237, 400–402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, B. D., &Gossman, J. R. (1970). Parallel processing of multidimensional stimuli.Perception & Psychophysics,8, 57–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., McLeod, P., &Dienes, Z. (1992a). Are direction and speed coded independently by the visual system? Evidence from visual search.Spatial Vision,6, 133–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., McLeod, P., &Dienes, Z. (1992b). Motion coherence and conjunction search: Implications for guided search theory.Perception & Psychophysics,51, 79–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli.Psychological Review,87, 272–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1989). Boundary conditions on parallel processing in human vision.Perception,18, 457–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., &Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity.Psychological Review,96, 433–458.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., &Humphreys, G. W. (1992). Beyond the search surface: Visual search and attentional engagement.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 578–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Zmura, M. (1991). Color in visual search.Vision Research,31, 951–966.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, R., Yund, E. W., &Nichols, D. R. (1987). Scanning the visual field without eye movements: A sex difference.Neuropsychologia,25, 637–644.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E., Jonides, J., &Wall, S. (1972). Parallel processing of multielement displays.Cognitive Psychology,3, 674–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E., Virzi, R. A., &Garbart, H. (1984). Searching for conjunctively defined targets.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,10, 32–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, J., &Zucker, S. (1993). The effect of contour closure on the rapid discrimination of two-dimensional shapes.Vision Research,33, 981–991.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T. (1992). Sensitivity of early human vision to 3-D orientation in line-drawings.Canadian Journal of Psychology,46, 143–169.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., &Rensink, R. A. (1990a). Scene based properties influence visual search.Science,247, 721–723.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., &Rensink, R. A. (1990b). Sensitivity to three-dimensional orientation in visual search.Psychological Science,1, 323–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., &Rensink, R. A. (1991). Preattentive recovery of three-dimensional orientation from line drawings.Psychological Review,98, 335–351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., &Rensink, R. A. (1992). An object completion process in early vision.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1263. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, W., &Babler, T. (1990). In search of depth.Perception & Psychophysics,48, 68–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., &Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task.Perception & Psychophysics,16, 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahle, M. (1990). Parallel, semi-parallel, and serial processing of visual hyperacuity.Human Vision & Electronic Imaging: Models, Methods, & Applications,1249, 147–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahle, M. (1991a). A new elementary feature of vision.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,32, 2151–2155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahle, M. (1991b). Parallel perception of vernier offsets, curvature, and chevrons in humans.Vision Research,31, 2149–2184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, S.-P., &Wu, P. (1989). Illusory conjunctions in the perception of Chinese characters.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 434–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. (1992). Is an object an object an object? Cognitive and neuropsychological investigations of domain specificity in visual object recognition.Current Directions in Psychological Science,1, 165–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, E. W., &Taylor, R. M. (1980). Visual search through color displays: Effects of target-background similarity and background uniformity.Perception & Psychophysics,27, 267–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., &Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 1030–1044.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, D. H., &Ward, P. A. (1991a). Asymmetries in oriented-line detection indicate two orthogonal filters in early vision.Proceedings of the Royal Society London: Series B,243, 75–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, D. H., &Ward, P. A. (1991b). Horizontal-vertical filters in early vision predict anomalous line-orientation frequencies.Proceedings of the Royal Society London: Series B,243, 83–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francolini, C. M., &Egeth, H. E. (1979). Perceptual selectivity is task dependent: The pop-out effect poops out.Perception & Psychophysics,25, 99–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman-Hill, S. R., &Wolfe, J. M. (1992). Activation vs. inhibition in visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1356. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman-Hill, S. R., &Wolfe, J. M. (in press). Second-order parallel processing: Visual search for the odd item in a subset.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance.

  • Geisler, W. S. (1989). Sequential ideal-observer analysis of visual discriminations.Psychological Review,96, 267–314.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grabowecky, M., &Khurana, B. (1990). Features were meant to be integrated.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,31, 105. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, N., Beck, J., &Sutter, A. (1992). Nonlinear processes in spatial-frequency channel models of perceived texture segregation: Effects of sign and amount of contrast.Vision Research,32, 719–743.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, N., Sutter, A., Venkatesan, C., &Humaran, M. (1992). Non-linear processes in perceived region segregation: Orientation selectivity of complex channels.Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics,12, 142–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M. A., Ball, K. K., Cissell, G. M., West, R. E., Whorley, K. D., &Edwards, J. D. (1993). Auditory distraction results in functional visual impairment for some older drivers.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1418. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, B. F., &Anderson, L. K. (1956). Color coding in a visual search task.Journal of Experimental Psychology,51, 19–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurnsey, R., &Browse, R. A. (1989). Asymmetries in visual texture discrimination.Spatial Vision,4, 31–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurnsey, R., Humphrey, G. K., &Kapitan, P. (1992). Parallel discrimination of subjective contours defined by offset gratings.Perception & Psychophysics,52, 263–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haenny, P. E., Maunsell, J. H. R., &Schiller, P. H. (1988). State dependent activity in monkey visual cortex: II. Visual and non-visual factors.Experimental Brain Research,69, 245–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haenny, P. E., &Schiller, P. H. (1988). State dependent activity in monkey visual cortex: I. Single cell activity in V1 and V4 on visual tasks.Experimental Brain Research,69, 225–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heathcote, A., &Mewhort, D. J. K. (1993). Representation and selection of relative position.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 488–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helmholtz, H. von (1962).Treatise on physiological optics (Vol. 3; J. P. C. Southall, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1866)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E. (1978). Search through a sequentially presented visual display.Perception & Psychophysics,23, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E. (1979). A two-stage model of visual search.Perception & Psychophysics,25, 319–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holliday, I. E., &Braddick, O. J. (1991). Pre-attentive detection of a target defined by stereoscopic slant.Perception,20, 355–362.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H., &Livingstone, M. S. (1987). Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18.Journal of Neuroscience,7, 3378–3415.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hummel, J. E., &Biederman, I. (1992). Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition.Psychological Review,99, 480–517.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., &Müller, H. (1993). Search via recursive rejection (SERR): A connectionist model of visual search.Cognitive Psychology,25, 43–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Quinlan, P. T., &Riddoch, M. J. (1989). Grouping processes in visual search: Effects with single and combined-feature targets.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,118, 258–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Intraub, H. (1985). Visual dissociation: An illusory conjunction of pictures and forms.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,11, 431–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. B., &Cohen, A. (1990). Dissociation of short- and long-range apparent motion in visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 317–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, W. A., Hawley, K. J., &Farnham, J. M. (1993). Novel popout: Empirical boundaries and tentative theory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 140–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., &Gleitman, H. (1972). A conceptual category effect in visual search: O as letter or digit.Perception & Psychophysics,12, 457–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., &Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention.Perception & Psychophysics,43, 346–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1984). A brief outline of the texton theory of human vision.Trends in Neuroscience,7, 41–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1986). Texton gradients: The texton theory revisited.Biological Cybernetics,54, 245–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B., &Bergen, J. R. (1983). Textons, the fundamental elements in preattentive vision and perceptions of textures.Bell Systems Technical Journal,62, 1619–1646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B., &Kröse, B. (1988). Features and spatial filters.Nature,333, 302–303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., &Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.),Varieties of attention (pp. 29–61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N. (1991). Repetition blindness and illusory conjunctions: Errors in binding visual types with visual tokens.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 30–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., &Driver, J. (1992). Objects, attributes, and visual attention: Which, what, and where.Current Directions in Psychological Science,1, 26–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., &Potter, M. C. (1989). Repetition blindness: The effects of stimulus modality and spatial displacement.Memory & Cognition,17, 117–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., &Potter, M. C. (1990). Repetition blindness: Levels of processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 30–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karni, A., &Sagi, D. (1990). Texture discrimination learning is specific for spatial location and background element orientation.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,31, 562. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Karni, A., &Sagi, D. (1992). Later but (almost) forever-The time course of learning of a visual skill.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1356. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Keeble, D. R., &Morgan, M. J. (1993). A linear systems approach to texture perception.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1237. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, P. L., Harrison, D. W., &Hodge, M. H. (1991). The category effect in visual selective attention.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,29, 71–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A. (1977). The role of structural redundancy in the perception of visual targets.Perception & Psychophysics,22, 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A. (1992). Attention.Annual Review of Psychology,43, 711–742.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A., &Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: “Top-down,” “bottom-up,” or “middle-out.”Perception & Psychophysics,25, 225–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search.Nature,334, 430–431.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knierim, J. J., &Van Essen, D. C. (1992). Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert monkey.Journal of Neurophysiology,67, 961–980.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kröse, B. A. J., &Julesz, B. (1989). The control and speed of shifts of attention.Vision Research,29, 1607–1619.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwak, H.-W., Dagenbach, D., &Egeth, H. [E.] (1991). Further evidence for a time-independent shift of the focus of attention.Perception & Psychophysics,49, 473–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landy, M. S., &Bergen, J. R. (1991). Texture segregation and orientation gradient.Vision Research,31, 679–691.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lennie, P., Trevarthen, C., Van Essen, D., &Wassle, H. (1990). Parallel processing of visual information. In L. Spillman & J. S. Werner (Eds.),Visual perception: The neurophysiological foundations (pp. 103–128). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis, N. K., &Charles, E. R. (1990). V4 responses to gratings defined by random dot motion.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,31, 90. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Luschow, A., &Nothdurft, H. C. (1993). Pop-out of orientation but not pop-out of motion at isoluminance.Vision Research,33, 91–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mach, E. (1959).The analysis of sensations (C. M. Williams, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1886)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackeben, M., &Nakayama, K. (1988). Fixation release facilitates rapid attentional shifts.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,29, 22. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Malik, J., &Perona, P. (1990). Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,7, 923–932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maijkovic, V., &Nakayama, K. (1992). Color and position priming in the deployment of attention.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1354. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Marendaz, C., Stivalet, P., Barraclough, L., &Walkowiac, P. (1993). Effect of gravitoinertial cues on visual search for orientation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 1266–1277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maunsell, J. H. R., Sclar, G., &Nealey, T. A. (1988). Task-specific signals in area V4 of monkey visual cortex.Society for Neuroscience Abstracts,14, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, P., Driver, J., &Crisp, J. (1988). Visual search for conjunctions of movement and form is parallel.Nature,332, 154–155.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, P., Driver, J., Dienes, Z., &Crisp, J. (1991). Filtering by movement in visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 55–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moraglia, G. (1989). Display organization and the detection of horizontal line segments.Perception & Psychophysics,45, 265–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, J., &Desimone, R. (1985). Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex.Science,229, 782–784.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mordkoff, J. T., &Yantis, S. (1993). Dividing attention between color and shape: Evidence of coactivation.Perception & Psychophysics,53, 357–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moser, M. C. (1991).The perception of multiple objects. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, A. L., &Sanchez, R. R. (1990). Critical color differences determined with a visual search task.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,7, 1209–1217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, A. L., Sanchez, R. R., &Hughes, T. C. (1990). Visual search for color differences with foveal and peripheral vision.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,7, 1995–2001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, K. (1990). The iconic bottleneck and the tenuous link between early visual processing and perception. In C. Blakemore (Ed.),Vision: Coding and efficiency (pp. 411–422). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, K., &Silverman, G. H. (1986). Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions.Nature,320, 264–265.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.Cognitive Psychology,9, 353–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton, Century, Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1990). Texture discrimination by cells in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus.Experimental Brain Research,82, 48–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1991a). Different effects from spatial frequency masking in texture segregation and texton detection tasks.Vision Research,31, 299–320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1991b). Texture segmentation and pop-out from orientation contrast.Vision Research,31, 1073–1078.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1993a). Matching luminance pop-out with that of orientation or motion.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1054. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1993b). The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues.Vision Research,33, 1937–1958.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C., &Li, C. Y. (1984). Representation of spatial details in textured patterns by cells of the cat striate cortex.Experimental Brain Research,57, 9–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, K. M., &Treisman, A. (1992).Shared orientation coding for lines, dot pairs, and edges. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • O’Neill, P., Wolfe, J. M., &Bilsky, A. B. (1993). Individual differences in visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1236. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Toole, A. J., &Walker, C. L. (1993). Disparity as a visual primitive: The competing role of surface percepts.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1187. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1987). Detecting conjunctions of color and form: Reassessing the serial search hypothesis.Perception A Psychophysics,41, 191–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1988). Cross-dimensional interaction and texture segregation.Perception & Psychophysics,43, 307–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavel, M., Econopouly, J., &Landy, M. S. (1992). The psychophysics of rapid visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,33, 1355. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterhans, E., Von der Heydt, R., &Baumgartner, G. (1986). Neuronal responses to illusory contour stimuli reveal stages of visual cortical processing. In J. D. Pettigrew, K. J. Sanderson, & W. R. Levick (Eds.),Visual neuroscience (pp. 343–351). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poisson, M. E., &Wilkinson, F. (1992). Distractor ratio and grouping processes in visual conjunction search.Perception,21, 21–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., &Pristach, E. A. (1989). Emergent features, attention, and perceptual glue in visual form perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception A Performance,15, 635–649.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., &Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of attention. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bowhuis (Eds.),Attention and performance X (pp. 55–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., &Keysar, B. (1989). Functional theory of illusory conjunctions and neon colors.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,118, 165–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, P. T., &Humphreys, G. W. (1987). Visual search for targets defined by combinations of color, shape, and size: An examination of the task constraints on feature and conjunction searches.Perception & Psychophysics,41, 455–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rafal, R. D., Calabresi, P. A., Brennan, C. W., &Sciolto, T. K. (1989). Saccade preparation to recently attended locations.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception A Performance,15, 673–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. (1988). Perception of shape from shading.Nature,331, 163–165.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval.Psychological Review,85, 59–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R., &Cavanagh, P. (1993). Processing of shadows at preattentive levels.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1288. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, L. C., Egly, R., Lamb, M. R., &Kerth, L. (1993). Spatial attention and cuing to global and local levels of hierarchical structure.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception A Performance,19, 471–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, L. C., &Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization.Cognitive Psychology,23, 299–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. (1974, January). The perception of disoriented figures.Scientific American, pp. 78–85.

  • Ross, W. D., Grossberg, S., &Mingolla, E. (1993). A neural model of visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1235. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, B. S., &Sagi, D. (1990). Spatial variability as a limiting factor in texture discrimination tasks: Implications for performance asymmetries.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,7, 1632–1643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, J. M., &Kanwisher, N. (1985). Topological perception: Holes in an experiment.Perception A Psychophysics,37, 179–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D. (1988). The combination of spatial frequency and orientation is effortlessly perceived.Perception A Psychophysics,43, 601–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D. (1990). Detection of an orientation singularity in Gabor textures: Effect of signal density and spatial-frequency.Vision Research,30, 1377–1388.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D., &Julesz, B. (1985a). Fast noninertial shifts of attention.Spatial Vision,1, 141–149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D., &Julesz, B. (1985b). “Where” and “what” in vision.Science,228, 1217–1219.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sandon, P. A., &Yanikoglu, B. A. (1990). Visual search as constraint propagation. InProceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 574–581). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., &Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention.Psychological Review,84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sekuler, R., &Ball, K. K. (1986). Visual localization: Age and practice.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,3, 864–868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smallman, H. S., &Boynton, R. M. (1990). Segregation of basic color in an information display.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,7, 1985–1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. L. (1962). Color coding and visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology,64, 434–440.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, H., Desimone, R., &Moran, J. (1988). Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance.Science,240, 338–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). High-speed scanning in human memory.Science,153, 652–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., &Mack, A. (1993). Perception of grouping based on motion without attention.Investigative Ophthalmology A Visual Science,34, 1233. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tadmor, Y., &Tolhurst, D. J. (1993). Both the phase and the amplitude spectrum may determine the appearance of natural images.Vision Research,33, 141–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S., &Badcock, D. (1988). Processing feature density in preat-tentive perception.Perception A Psychophysics,44, 551–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity.Perception A Psychophysics,50, 184–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form.Perception & Psychophysics,51, 599–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, J. P., &Gille, J. (1979). Bandwidths of orientation channels in human vision.Journal of the Optical Society of America,69, 652–660.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., &Bayus, S. C. (1987). Individual difference in selective attention: The relation of priming and interference to cognitive failure.Personality A Individual Differences,8, 667–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., &Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 492–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Driver, J., &Weaver, B. (1991). Object centered inhibition of return of visual attention.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,43A, 289–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T. (1971). A note on the identifiability of parallel and serial processes.Perception A Psychophysics,10, 161–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T. (1976). Serial and within-stage independent parallel model equivalence on the minimum completion time.Journal of Mathematical Psychology,14, 219–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T. (1990). Serial and parallel processing: Sometimes they look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and should) be distinguished.Psychological Science,1, 46–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1985). Preattentive processing in vision.Computer Vision, Graphics, & Image Processing,31, 156–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1986a, May). Features and objects in visual processing.Scientific American, pp. 114B–125.

  • Treisman, A. (1986b). Properties, parts, and objects. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufmann, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.),Handbook of human perception and performance (pp. 35.1–35.70). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: The 14th Bartlett Memorial Lecture.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,40A, 201–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention.Cognitive Psychology,12, 97–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries.Psychological Review,95, 15–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 459–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Schmidt, H. (1982). Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects.Cognitive Psychology,14, 107–141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Souther, J. (1986). Illusory words: The roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to form words.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,12, 3–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., Vieira, A., &Hayes, A. (1992). Automaticity and preattentive processing.American Journal of Psychology,105, 341–362.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsotsos, J. K. (1990). Analyzing vision at the complexity level.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,13, 423–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Essen, D. C., &Maunsell, J. H. R. (1983). Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex.Trends in Neuroscience,6, 370–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vieira, A., &Treisman, A. (1988, November).Automatic search: Changing perceptions or procedures? Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago.

  • Virzi, R. A., &Egeth, H. E. (1984). Is meaning implicated in illusory contours?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,10, 573–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von der Heydt, R., &Dursteler, M. R. (1993). Visual search: Monkeys detect conjunctions as fast as features.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1288. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Von der Heydt, R., Peterhans, E., &Baumgartner, G. (1984). Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses.Science,224, 1260–1262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voorhees, H., &Poggio, T. (1988). Computing texture boundaries from images.Nature,333, 364–367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q., &Cavanagh, P. (1993). Acquired familiarity effects in visual search with Chinese characters.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1236. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q., Cavanagh, P., &Green, M. (1992). Familiarity and pop-out in visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 1262. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, R., &McClelland, J. L. (1989). Conjunctive search for one and two identical targets.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 664–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, J. M., Levi, D. M., &Aitsebaomo, A. P. (1992). Spatial localization without visual references.Vision Research,32, 513–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, H. R. (1986). Responses of spatial mechanisms can explain hyperacuity.Vision Research,26, 453–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, H. R., Levi, D., Maffei, L., Rovamo, J., &De Valois, R. (1990). The perception of form: Retina to striate cortex. In L. Spillman & J. S. Werner (Eds.),Visual perception: The neurophysiological foundations (pp. 231–272). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1986). Stereopsis and binocular rivalry.Psychological Review,93, 269–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1992). “Effortless” texture segmentation and “parallel” visual search are nor the same thing.Vision Research,32, 757–763.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Visual search in continuous, naturalistic stimuli.Vision Research,34, 1187–1195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Bose, M. (1991).Visual search for the “medium” stimulus. Unpublished research report. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Cave, K. R. (1989). Deploying visual attention: The guided search model. In T. Troscianko & A. Blake (Eds.),AI and the eye (pp. 79–103). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., &Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 419–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Chun, M. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1993). Making use of texton gradients: Visual search and texton grouping exploit the same parallel processes in different ways.Spatial Vision,7, 90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M.,Chun, M. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (in press). Making use of texton gradients: Visual search and perceptual grouping exploit the same parallel processes in different ways. In T. Papathomas & A. Gorea (Eds.),Unking psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computational vision: A volume in honor of Beta Julesz.

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Franzel, S. L. (1988). Binocularity and visual search.Perception & Psychophysics,44, 81–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1990, November).Must we attend to every abrupt onset? Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans.

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992a). On the role of symmetry in visual search.Psychological Science,3, 194–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992b). Visual search for orientation: The role of angular relations between targets and distrac-tors.Spatial Vision,6, 199–208.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Friedman-Hill, S. R., &Bilsky, A. B. (1994). Parallel processing of part-whole information in visual search tasks.Perception & Psychophysics,55, 537–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Friedman-Hill, S. R., Stewart, M. I., &O’Con-nell, K. M. (1992). The role of categorization in visual search for orientation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 34–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., &Pokorny, C. W. (1990). Inhibitory tagging in visual search: A failure to replicate.Perception & Psychophysics,48, 357–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Yee, A., &Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992). Curvature is a basic feature for visual search.Perception,21, 465–480.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M.,Yu, K. P.,Pruszenski, A. D., &Cave, K. R. (1988, November).A preattentive feature process can execute only one command at a time. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago.

  • Wolfe, J. M., Yu, K. P., Stewart, M. I., Shorter, A. D., Friedman-Hill, S. R., &Cave, K. R. (1990). Limitations on the parallel guidance of visual search: Color X color and orientation x orientation conjunctions.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 879–892.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture.Current Directions in Psychological Science,2, 156–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., &Johnson, D. N. (1990). Mechanisms of attentional priority.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 812–825.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., &Jones, E. (1991). Mechanisms of attentional selection: Temporally modulated priority tags.Perception & Psychophysics,50, 166–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., &Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 121–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yund, E. W., Efron, R., &Nichols, D. R. (1990). Detectability gradients as a function of target location.Brain & Cognition,12, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S. M. (1978). Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in Rhesus monkey prestriate visual cortex.Journal of Physiology,277, 273–290.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S. M., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Friston, K. J., Ken-nard, C., &Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex.Journal of Neuroscience,11, 641–649.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, W., Chen, L., &Zhang, X. (1992). Topological perception: Holes in illusory conjunction and visual search.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,33, 958. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, W., Zhang, X., &Chen, L. (1993). Shape transformation in illusory conjunctions.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,34, 1082. (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Zohary, E., &Hochstein, S. (1989). How serial is serial processing in vision?Perception,18, 191–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references