Why skill matters - PubMed
Why skill matters
Okihide Hikosaka et al. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Sep.
Abstract
Maximizing rewards per unit time is ideal for success and survival in humans and animals. This goal can be approached by speeding up behavior aiming at rewards and this is done most efficiently by acquiring skills. Importantly, reward-directed skills consist of two components: finding a good object (i.e., object skill) and acting on the object (i.e., action skill), which occur sequentially. Recent studies suggest that object skill is based on high-capacity memory for object-value associations. When a learned object is encountered the corresponding memory is quickly expressed as a value-based gaze bias, leading to the automatic acquisition or avoidance of the object. Object skill thus plays a crucial role in increasing rewards per unit time.
Keywords: Object–value memory; automaticity; gaze; reward delay; saccade; stable value.
Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Figures

Two kinds of reward delay affecting reward value – hypothetical tasks. A: External delay. In front of you (as a subject) are two buttons, E and L. If you press button E, you will get a drop of juice 1 second later. If you press button L, you will get a drop of juice 5 seconds later. After you have consumed the drop of juice, you can press the button again. Shown here are two extreme cases: you continue to press button E, or continue to press button L. The amount of reward per unit time is larger in the former case. B: Internal delay. In front of you are two buttons, F and S. They are located differently so that you can reach button F in 1 second and button S in 5 seconds. Once you press either button, you will get a drop of juice 1 second later.

Internal reward delay can be shortened. A: Motivation or skill shortens internal reward delay by shortening own behavior. B: Two kinds of skill that shorten reward delay.

Fractal objects that one monkey experienced for a long time with biased rewards (n=288). In each row of eight fractals, the left four fractals were high-valued objects (consistently associated with a large reward) and the right four fractals were low-valued objects (consistently associated with a small reward). Reproduced from [65].

Object skill expressed as automatic gaze bias. A: Procedure for learning stable values of visual objects. Half of the objects were associated with a reward (high-valued objects) and the other associated with no (or a small) reward (low-valued objects). B: Free viewing procedure for testing the value-based gaze bias. On each trial, 4 fractal objects were chosen pseudo-randomly from a set of 8 learned objects (A), were presented simultaneously, and the monkey freely looked at them, but no reward was delivered. Shown here are examples of saccade trajectories. The monkey tended to look at high-valued objects (denoted as ‘H’). (C, D) The percentage of the gaze duration on each object before (C) and after (D) the long-term learning. Red and blue indicated high- and low-valued objects. Reproduced from [63].

Basal ganglia circuit that supports object skill. Neurons in the monkey CDt receives inputs from the temporal visual cortices and respond to visual objects differentially. Their responses are modulated by the stable (not flexible) values of the visual objects. Neurons in the SNr, which receive inputs from the CDt directly or indirectly, categorize visual objects into high- and low-valued objects. Their stable value signals are then sent to the SC, thereby biasing gaze toward high-valued objects. Arrows indicate excitatory connections (or effects). Lines with circular dots indicate inhibitory connections. Solid and hatched lines indicate direct and indirect connections, respectively.
Similar articles
-
Pomper JK, Spadacenta S, Bunjes F, Arnstein D, Giese MA, Thier P. Pomper JK, et al. J Neurophysiol. 2020 Sep 1;124(3):941-961. doi: 10.1152/jn.00234.2020. Epub 2020 Aug 12. J Neurophysiol. 2020. PMID: 32783574
-
Supplementary motor area encodes reward expectancy in eye-movement tasks.
Campos M, Breznen B, Bernheim K, Andersen RA. Campos M, et al. J Neurophysiol. 2005 Aug;94(2):1325-35. doi: 10.1152/jn.00022.2005. Epub 2005 Apr 20. J Neurophysiol. 2005. PMID: 15843484
-
Object-finding skill created by repeated reward experience.
Ghazizadeh A, Griggs W, Hikosaka O. Ghazizadeh A, et al. J Vis. 2016 Aug 1;16(10):17. doi: 10.1167/16.10.17. J Vis. 2016. PMID: 27564993 Free PMC article.
-
Affective and motivational control of vision.
Vuilleumier P. Vuilleumier P. Curr Opin Neurol. 2015 Feb;28(1):29-35. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000159. Curr Opin Neurol. 2015. PMID: 25490197 Review.
-
Visual processing, learning and feedback in the primate eye movement system.
Trommershäuser J, Glimcher PW, Gegenfurtner KR. Trommershäuser J, et al. Trends Neurosci. 2009 Nov;32(11):583-90. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.07.004. Epub 2009 Sep 2. Trends Neurosci. 2009. PMID: 19729211 Review.
Cited by
-
Neurons in the primate dorsal striatum signal the uncertainty of object-reward associations.
White JK, Monosov IE. White JK, et al. Nat Commun. 2016 Sep 14;7:12735. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12735. Nat Commun. 2016. PMID: 27623750 Free PMC article.
-
Tian W, Chen S. Tian W, et al. Front Neurol. 2021 Feb 25;12:616820. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.616820. eCollection 2021. Front Neurol. 2021. PMID: 33716924 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Ghazizadeh A, Hikosaka O. Ghazizadeh A, et al. Sci Adv. 2021 May 12;7(20):eabe0693. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abe0693. Print 2021 May. Sci Adv. 2021. PMID: 33980480 Free PMC article.
-
Value-based attentional capture influences context-dependent decision-making.
Itthipuripat S, Cha K, Rangsipat N, Serences JT. Itthipuripat S, et al. J Neurophysiol. 2015 Jul;114(1):560-9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00343.2015. Epub 2015 May 20. J Neurophysiol. 2015. PMID: 25995350 Free PMC article.
-
Prefrontal cortex encodes value pop-out in visual search.
Abbaszadeh M, Panjehpour A, Amin Alemohammad SM, Ghavampour A, Ghazizadeh A. Abbaszadeh M, et al. iScience. 2023 Aug 3;26(9):107521. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.107521. eCollection 2023 Sep 15. iScience. 2023. PMID: 37680488 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Newell A, Rosenbloom PS. Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In: Anderson JR, editor. Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition. Erlbaum; 1981. pp. 1–55.
-
- Ericsson KA, Lehmann AC. Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual review of psychology. 1996;47:273–305. - PubMed
-
- Gurven M, et al. How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life span. Journal of human evolution. 2006;51:454–470. - PubMed
-
- Helton WS. Expertise acquisition as sustained learning in humans and other animals: commonalities across species. Animal cognition. 2008;11:99–107. - PubMed
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources