Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel - PubMed
- ️Wed Jan 01 2014
. 2014 Oct 7;111(40):14388-93.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1409643111. Epub 2014 Sep 22.
Andrew P Clark 1 , Steven C Josephson 2 , Adam H Boyette 3 , Innes C Cuthill 4 , Ruby L Fried 5 , Mhairi A Gibson 6 , Barry S Hewlett 3 , Mark Jamieson 7 , William Jankowiak 8 , P Lynne Honey 9 , Zejun Huang 10 , Melissa A Liebert 5 , Benjamin G Purzycki 11 , John H Shaver 11 , J Josh Snodgrass 5 , Richard Sosis 11 , Lawrence S Sugiyama 5 , Viren Swami 12 , Douglas W Yu 13 , Yangke Zhao 10 , Ian S Penton-Voak 14
Affiliations
- PMID: 25246593
- PMCID: PMC4210032
- DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1409643111
Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel
Isabel M Scott et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014.
Abstract
A large literature proposes that preferences for exaggerated sex typicality in human faces (masculinity/femininity) reflect a long evolutionary history of sexual and social selection. This proposal implies that dimorphism was important to judgments of attractiveness and personality in ancestral environments. It is difficult to evaluate, however, because most available data come from large-scale, industrialized, urban populations. Here, we report the results for 12 populations with very diverse levels of economic development. Surprisingly, preferences for exaggerated sex-specific traits are only found in the novel, highly developed environments. Similarly, perceptions that masculine males look aggressive increase strongly with development and, specifically, urbanization. These data challenge the hypothesis that facial dimorphism was an important ancestral signal of heritable mate value. One possibility is that highly developed environments provide novel opportunities to discern relationships between facial traits and behavior by exposing individuals to large numbers of unfamiliar faces, revealing patterns too subtle to detect with smaller samples.
Keywords: aggression; cross-cultural; evolution; facial attractiveness; stereotyping.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Figures

Examples of stimuli used. A European female composite (Upper) and an East Asian male composite (Lower) are shown. Masculinized stimuli (Left) and feminized stimuli (Right) are shown.

(A) Preferences for sex dimorphism in female faces, by group. Blue sections indicate the proportion of a group that chose masculinized faces as most attractive, white sections indicate the proportion that chose neutral faces, and pink sections indicate the proportion that chose feminized faces. (B) Preferences for sex dimorphism in male faces, by group. Blue sections indicate the proportion of a group that chose masculinized faces as most attractive, white sections indicate the proportion that chose neutral faces, and pink sections indicate the proportion that chose feminized faces.

Preferences for sex dimorphism in male faces by level of disease burden. Female preferences for masculinity in male faces by group, expressed as an average [participants’ choices for most attractive male faces were recorded as +60 (feminine), 0 (average), or −60 (masculine)], plotted against years lost to infectious disease in local populations (log-transformed). Preferences for masculinity decrease as the disease burden increases.

Male faces perceived as most aggressive-looking, by group. Blue sections indicate the proportion of a group that chose masculinized faces as most aggressive, white sections indicate the proportion that chose neutral faces, and pink sections indicate the proportion that chose feminized faces.

Masculinity of male faces perceived as most aggressive-looking, by level of urbanization. Average levels of masculinity in the male faces chosen as most aggressive-looking by group, plotted against level of urbanization, are shown. Participants in urban environments were more likely to choose masculine faces when asked to choose the most aggressive-looking face.
Similar articles
-
Dixson BJ, Sulikowski D, Gouda-Vossos A, Rantala MJ, Brooks RC. Dixson BJ, et al. J Evol Biol. 2016 Nov;29(11):2311-2320. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12958. Epub 2016 Aug 22. J Evol Biol. 2016. PMID: 27488414
-
Oh D, Grant-Villegas N, Todorov A. Oh D, et al. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2020 Nov;46(11):1328-1343. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000858. Epub 2020 Aug 6. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2020. PMID: 32757588
-
Nakamura K, Watanabe K. Nakamura K, et al. Sci Rep. 2020 Oct 6;10(1):16588. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73472-8. Sci Rep. 2020. PMID: 33024137 Free PMC article.
-
Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research.
Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM. Little AC, et al. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011 Jun 12;366(1571):1638-59. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011. PMID: 21536551 Free PMC article. Review.
-
The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty.
Rhodes G. Rhodes G. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57:199-226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006. PMID: 16318594 Review.
Cited by
-
Birth size and morphological femininity in adult women.
Żelaźniewicz A, Nowak J, Pawłowski B. Żelaźniewicz A, et al. BMC Evol Biol. 2020 Aug 15;20(1):102. doi: 10.1186/s12862-020-01670-z. BMC Evol Biol. 2020. PMID: 32799803 Free PMC article.
-
Replication, Communication, and the Population Dynamics of Scientific Discovery.
McElreath R, Smaldino PE. McElreath R, et al. PLoS One. 2015 Aug 26;10(8):e0136088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136088. eCollection 2015. PLoS One. 2015. PMID: 26308448 Free PMC article.
-
Domestic violence shapes Colombian women's partner choices.
Borras-Guevara ML, Batres C, Perrett DI. Borras-Guevara ML, et al. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2017;71(12):175. doi: 10.1007/s00265-017-2405-2. Epub 2017 Nov 19. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2017. PMID: 29200603 Free PMC article.
-
Psychobiological Responses Reveal Audiovisual Noise Differentially Challenges Speech Recognition.
Bidelman GM, Brown B, Mankel K, Nelms Price C. Bidelman GM, et al. Ear Hear. 2020 Mar/Apr;41(2):268-277. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000755. Ear Hear. 2020. PMID: 31283529 Free PMC article.
-
Meeker TJ, Veldhuijzen DS, Keaser ML, Gullapalli RP, Greenspan JD. Meeker TJ, et al. Front Neurosci. 2020 Dec 22;14:594588. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.594588. eCollection 2020. Front Neurosci. 2020. PMID: 33414702 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Ryan MJ, Keddy-Hector A. Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases. Am Nat. 1992;139(S1):S4–S35.
-
- Folstad I, Karter AJ. Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. Am Nat. 1992;139(3):603–622.
-
- Johnstone RA. Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: Reviewing the evidence. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 1995;70(1):1–65. - PubMed
-
- Perrett DI, et al. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature. 1998;394(6696):884–887. - PubMed
-
- Johnston VS. Female facial beauty: The fertility hypothesis. Pragmatics & Cognition. 2000;8(1):107–122.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources